You cannot speak and listen at the same time: a probabilistic model of turn-taking
暂无分享,去创建一个
Giovanni Pezzulo | Haris Dindo | Francesco Donnarumma | Pierpaolo Iodice | G. Pezzulo | Francesco Donnarumma | H. Dindo | P. Iodice
[1] S. Duncan,et al. Some Signals and Rules for Taking Speaking Turns in Conversations , 1972 .
[2] Adam N. Sanborn. Types of approximation for probabilistic cognition: Sampling and variational , 2017, Brain and Cognition.
[3] Björn Lindblom,et al. Explaining Phonetic Variation: A Sketch of the H&H Theory , 1990 .
[4] Roger K. Moore. PRESENCE: A Human-Inspired Architecture for Speech-Based Human-Machine Interaction , 2007, IEEE Transactions on Computers.
[5] Y. Paulignan,et al. An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.
[6] Karl J. Friston,et al. Active Inference: A Process Theory , 2017, Neural Computation.
[7] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Shared action spaces: a basis function framework for social re-calibration of sensorimotor representations supporting joint action , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[8] 付伶俐. 打磨Using Language,倡导新理念 , 2014 .
[9] Björn Granström,et al. Multimodality in Language and Speech Systems , 2002 .
[10] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Prefrontal Goal Codes Emerge as Latent States in Probabilistic Value Learning , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] E. Schegloff,et al. A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation , 1974 .
[12] Michael C. Frank,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Using Speakers ’ Referential Intentions to Model Early Cross-Situational Word Learning , 2022 .
[13] G. Dell,et al. Adapting production to comprehension: The explicit mention of instruments , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[14] G. Pezzulo,et al. Avoiding Accidents at the Champagne Reception , 2017, Psychological science.
[15] Charles Kemp,et al. How to Grow a Mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction , 2011, Science.
[16] Kensy Cooperrider. Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition and interaction , 2009 .
[17] Kristinn R. Thórisson,et al. Natural Turn-Taking Needs No Manual: Computational Theory and Model, from Perception to Action , 2002 .
[18] Stuart J. Russell,et al. Dynamic bayesian networks: representation, inference and learning , 2002 .
[19] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. What should I do next? Using shared representations to solve interaction problems , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] J. Kalaska,et al. Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action , 2005, Neuron.
[21] Jan-Peter de Holger N. J. Ruiter,et al. Projecting the End of a Speaker's Turn: A Cognitive Cornerstone of Conversation , 2006 .
[22] Stefan Kopp,et al. A model for production, perception, and acquisition of actions in face-to-face communication , 2010, Cognitive Processing.
[23] Cordula Vesper,et al. Making oneself predictable: reduced temporal variability facilitates joint action coordination , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] Martin J. Pickering,et al. Edinburgh Research Explorer A cognitive architecture for the coordination of utterances , 2022 .
[25] Karl J. Friston,et al. Action perception as hypothesis testing , 2017, Cortex.
[26] C. Hofsten. An action perspective on motor development , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] R. Mccall,et al. The Genetic and Environmental Origins of Learning Abilities and Disabilities in the Early School , 2007, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[28] Margaret Wilson,et al. An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[29] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. A Programmer-Interpreter Neural Network Architecture for Prefrontal Cognitive Control , 2015, Int. J. Neural Syst..
[30] H. Bekkering,et al. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] Noah D. Goodman,et al. A rational account of pedagogical reasoning: Teaching by, and learning from, examples , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.
[32] Yiannis Demiris,et al. Hierarchical attentive multiple models for execution and recognition of actions , 2006, Robotics Auton. Syst..
[33] Rebecca J. Brand,et al. Infants prefer motionese to adult-directed action. , 2008, Developmental science.
[34] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Problem Solving as Probabilistic Inference with Subgoaling: Explaining Human Successes and Pitfalls in the Tower of Hanoi , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[35] H. H. Clark,et al. Audience Design in Meaning and Reference , 1982 .
[36] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Evidence for ‘motionese’: modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action , 2002 .
[37] S. Levinson. On the Human "Interaction Engine" , 2020, Roots of Human Sociality.
[38] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. The intentional stance as structure learning: a computational perspective on mindreading , 2015, Biological Cybernetics.
[39] Karl J. Friston,et al. Action and behavior: a free-energy formulation , 2010, Biological Cybernetics.
[40] P. Kuhl,et al. Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. , 1997, Science.
[41] Arnaud Revel,et al. Emergence of structured interactions: From a theoretical model to pragmatic robotics , 2009, Neural Networks.
[42] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Sensorimotor Coarticulation in the Execution and Recognition of Intentional Actions , 2017, Front. Psychol..
[43] M. Pickering,et al. Why is conversation so easy? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] Daniel A. Braun,et al. Signaling equilibria in sensorimotor interactions , 2015, Cognition.
[45] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Intentional strategies that make co-actors more predictable: the case of signaling. , 2013 .
[46] M. Pickering,et al. An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[47] Chrystopher L. Nehaniv,et al. Emergent dynamics of turn-taking interaction in drumming games with a humanoid robot , 2008, RO-MAN 2008 - The 17th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.
[48] J. F. Soechting,et al. Coarticulation in Fluent Fingerspelling , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[49] Daniel A. Braun,et al. Thermodynamics as a theory of decision-making with information-processing costs , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.
[50] Karl J. Friston. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[51] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Bayesian brain : probabilistic approaches to neural coding , 2006 .
[52] Ivan Toni,et al. Neural Correlates of Intentional Communication , 2010, Front. Neurosci..
[53] Petra Wagner,et al. Pitch and duration as a basis for entrainment of overlapped speech onsets , 2013, INTERSPEECH.
[54] R. Johansson,et al. Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.
[55] B. Repp,et al. Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.
[56] M. Pickering,et al. Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[57] Wolfgang Maass,et al. Neural Dynamics as Sampling: A Model for Stochastic Computation in Recurrent Networks of Spiking Neurons , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[58] G. Pezzulo,et al. Human Sensorimotor Communication: A Theory of Signaling in Online Social Interactions , 2013, PloS one.
[59] P. Kay,et al. Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[60] K. Doya,et al. A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[61] A. Glenberg,et al. Action-based language: A theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production , 2012, Cortex.
[62] L. Fadiga,et al. Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[63] D M Wolpert,et al. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.
[64] Karl J. Friston,et al. Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.
[65] Lilla Magyari,et al. Prediction of Turn-Ends Based on Anticipation of Upcoming Words , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[66] Martin J. Pickering,et al. The use of content and timing to predict turn transitions , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[67] Christopher M. Bishop,et al. Pattern Recognition and Machine Learning (Information Science and Statistics) , 2006 .
[68] M. Heldner. Detection thresholds for gaps, overlaps, and no-gap-no-overlaps. , 2011, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[69] Thomas L. Griffiths,et al. One and Done? Optimal Decisions From Very Few Samples , 2014, Cogn. Sci..
[70] S. Levinson,et al. Brain Mechanisms Underlying Human Communication , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[71] P. Lieberman. Some Effects of Semantic and Grammatical Context on the Production and Perception of Speech , 1963 .
[72] P. Berkes,et al. Statistically Optimal Perception and Learning: from Behavior to Neural Representations , 2022 .
[73] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Shared Representations as Coordination Tools for Interaction , 2011 .
[74] Natalie Sebanz,et al. Prediction in Joint Action: What, When, and Where , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[75] G. Pezzulo. Tracing the Roots of Cognition in Predictive Processing , 2017 .
[76] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Studying mirror mechanisms within generative and predictive architectures for joint action , 2013, Cortex.
[77] S. Levinson. Turn-taking in Human Communication – Origins and Implications for Language Processing , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[78] Kristinn R. Thórisson,et al. Learning Smooth, Human-Like Turntaking in Realtime Dialogue , 2008, IVA.
[79] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. The “Interaction Engine”: A Common Pragmatic Competence Across Linguistic and Nonlinguistic Interactions , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.
[80] Moritz M. Daum,et al. The use of intonation for turn anticipation in observed conversations without visual signals as source of information , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[81] Dan Jurafsky,et al. Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[82] Karl J. Friston,et al. A Duet for one , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.
[83] Karl J. Friston,et al. Active inference, communication and hermeneutics , 2015, Cortex.
[84] Karl J. Friston. Hierarchical Models in the Brain , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[85] Yiannis Demiris,et al. Towards Active Event Recognition , 2013, IJCAI.
[86] M. Candidi,et al. Kinematics fingerprints of leader and follower role-taking during cooperative joint actions , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[87] G. Dell,et al. Effect of Ambiguity and Lexical Availability on Syntactic and Lexical Production , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[88] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[89] Karl J. Friston,et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2022 .
[90] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Proceedings of the Twenty-Second International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence Motor Simulation via Coupled Internal Models Using Sequential Monte Carlo , 2022 .
[91] G. Csibra,et al. Natural pedagogy , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[92] Adam N. Sanborn,et al. Bayesian Brains without Probabilities , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[93] Jill Gilkerson,et al. A Social Feedback Loop for Speech Development and Its Reduction in Autism , 2014, Psychological science.
[94] Mattias Heldner,et al. Pauses, gaps and overlaps in conversations , 2010, J. Phonetics.
[95] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Nonparametric Problem-Space Clustering: Learning Efficient Codes for Cognitive Control Tasks , 2016, Entropy.
[96] Michael J. Richardson,et al. Strategic communication and behavioral coupling in asymmetric joint action , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[97] L. Craighero,et al. Leadership in Orchestra Emerges from the Causal Relationships of Movement Kinematics , 2012, PloS one.
[98] J. Kalaska,et al. Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.
[99] Azwirman Gusrialdi,et al. Modeling inter-human movement coordination: synchronization governs joint task dynamics , 2012, Biological Cybernetics.
[100] Martin V. Butz,et al. Toward a Unified Sub-symbolic Computational Theory of Cognition , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[101] Radford M. Neal. Pattern Recognition and Machine Learning , 2007, Technometrics.
[102] Yiannis Demiris,et al. Echo State Gaussian Process , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks.
[103] Simon J. Godsill,et al. On sequential Monte Carlo sampling methods for Bayesian filtering , 2000, Stat. Comput..
[104] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control , 2022 .
[105] Mattias Heldner,et al. Very Short Utterances and Timing in Turn-Taking , 2011, INTERSPEECH.
[106] G. Pezzulo,et al. Navigating the Affordance Landscape: Feedback Control as a Process Model of Behavior and Cognition , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[107] Richard B Ivry,et al. Temporal Control and Coordination: The Multiple Timer Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.
[108] József Fiser,et al. Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks of an Optimal Internal Model of the Environment , 2011, Science.
[109] Nando de Freitas,et al. An Introduction to Sequential Monte Carlo Methods , 2001, Sequential Monte Carlo Methods in Practice.
[110] G. Pezzulo,et al. Interactional leader–follower sensorimotor communication strategies during repetitive joint actions , 2015, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.
[111] Karl J. Friston,et al. Action understanding and active inference , 2011, Biological Cybernetics.
[112] S. Feldstein,et al. Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing in development. , 2001, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.