Easy-to-hard effects in perceptual learning depend upon the degree to which initial trials are “easy”
暂无分享,去创建一个
Matthew G. Wisniewski | Eduardo Mercado | Alexandria C Zakrzewski | Matthew G Wisniewski | Barbara A Church | E. Mercado | B. Church | Milen L. Radell | Alexandria C. Zakrzewski | Milen L Radell
[1] Matthew G. Wisniewski,et al. Individual differences during acquisition predict shifts in generalization , 2014, Behavioural Processes.
[2] Jessica C. Lee,et al. Rule-based generalization and peak shift in the presence of simple relational rules , 2018, PloS one.
[3] Eduardo Mercado,et al. The easy-to-hard effect in human (Homo sapiens) and rat (Rattus norvegicus) auditory identification. , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.
[4] Kristin J. Van Engen,et al. Listening effort and accented speech , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[5] E. Mercado,et al. Evoked-potential changes following discrimination learning involving complex sounds , 2012, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[6] N. Mackintosh,et al. Associative learning and elemental representation: II. Generalization and discrimination , 2002, Animal learning & behavior.
[7] M. Mozer,et al. When does fading enhance perceptual category learning? , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[8] Ediz Sohoglu,et al. Less Is More: Latent Learning Is Maximized by Shorter Training Sessions in Auditory Perceptual Learning , 2012, PloS one.
[9] Matthew G. Wisniewski,et al. Temporal dynamics in auditory perceptual learning: impact of sequencing and incidental learning. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[10] N. Mackintosh,et al. Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning , 1971 .
[11] Eduardo Mercado,et al. Learning to discriminate frequency modulation rate can benefit and worsen pitch acuity. , 2014, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[12] Robert L. Goldstone,et al. The benefits of interleaved and blocked study: Different tasks benefit from different schedules of study , 2014, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[13] Matthew G. Wisniewski,et al. Benefits of fading in perceptual learning are driven by more than dimensional attention , 2017, PloS one.
[14] B. Wright,et al. Enhancing Perceptual Learning by Combining Practice with Periods of Additional Sensory Stimulation , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] Christopher B Sturdy,et al. Predicting shifts in generalization gradients with perceptrons , 2011, Learning & Behavior.
[16] Robert L. Goldstone,et al. Definition , 1960, A Philosopher Looks at Sport.
[17] María del Carmen Sanjuán,et al. An easy-to-hard effect after nonreinforced preexposure in a sweetness discrimination , 2014, Learning & behavior.
[18] J. Kruschke,et al. ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.
[19] K. Spence. The differential response in animals to stimuli varying within a single dimension. , 1937 .
[20] Matthew G. Wisniewski,et al. Transfer Effects in the Vocal Imitation of Speech and Song , 2013 .
[21] W. James,et al. The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .
[22] S. Hochstein,et al. The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] H. Levitt. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[24] Sygal Amitay,et al. A comparison of adaptive procedures for rapid and reliable threshold assessment and training in naive listeners. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[25] L. Saksida. Effects of similarity and experience on discrimination learning: a nonassociative connectionist model of perceptual learning. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.