Overt and covert identification of fragmented objects inferred from performance and electrophysiological measures.

The authors investigated visual processing leading to object identification by manipulating the number of fragments and nature of the study. During the study, participants either named or drew objects in Experiment 1 and drew them all in Experiment 2. During the test, participants made an identification judgment at each of 6 different fragmentation levels for studied and new objects. Fewer fragments were needed to identify studied than unstudied objects. Reaction times were faster for studied than unstudied objects both at identification and at the preceding level. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to unidentified objects were characterized by a late negativity in contrast to a positivity to identified objects. ERPs to studied but not to new objects contained a smaller and later version of the identification positivity at level just prior to identification, which was not due to differential response confidence. Much covert visual analysis and even object identification may precede overt identification, depending on the nature of prior experience.

[1]  K. Whitfield,et al.  Age cohort differences in the ability to perform closure on degraded figures. , 1992, Experimental aging research.

[2]  Joan Gay Snodgrass,et al.  Fragmenting pictures on the apple macintosh computer for experimental and clinical applications , 1987 .

[3]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Repetition priming and the time course of object recognition: an fMRI study. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[4]  R. Leeper A Study of a Neglected Portion of the Field of Learning—the Development of Sensory Organization , 1935 .

[5]  Elizabeth K. Warrington,et al.  Visual Apperceptive Agnosia: A Clinico-Anatomical Study of Three Cases , 1988, Cortex.

[6]  S. Howard Bartley,et al.  Introduction to Perception , 1980 .

[7]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Effects of aging on perceptual closure. , 1988, The American journal of psychology.

[8]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[9]  A. Treisman Perceiving and re-perceiving objects. , 1992, The American psychologist.

[10]  A. Treisman Perceiving and re-perceiving objects. , 1992, The American psychologist.

[11]  A. Taylor,et al.  The contribution of the right parietal lobe to object recognition. , 1973, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[12]  E. Warrington,et al.  Visual Object Recognition in Patients with Right-Hemisphere Lesions: Axes or Features? , 1986, Perception.

[13]  A. Parkin,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory and the automatid effortful distinction , 1990 .

[14]  J H Maunsell,et al.  The Brain's Visual World: Representation of Visual Targets in Cerebral Cortex , 1995, Science.

[15]  R. Desimone,et al.  Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. , 1994, Science.

[16]  S. Pitzalis,et al.  Identification of fragmented pictures in patients with brain damage. , 1998, Applied Neuropsychology.

[17]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[18]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.

[19]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[20]  O. Reiser,et al.  Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .

[21]  H. Roediger Implicit memory in normal human subjects , 1993 .

[22]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Perceptual identification of visually degraded stimuli , 1986 .

[23]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Priming effects in picture fragment completion: support for the perceptual closure hypothesis. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[25]  R. Parasuraman The attentive brain , 1998 .

[26]  J. Morton,et al.  The effects of priming on picture recognition. , 1982, British journal of psychology.

[27]  K. Henke,et al.  Subliminal Perception of Pictures in the Right Hemisphere , 1993, Consciousness and Cognition.

[28]  M. Tarr,et al.  Visual Object Recognition , 1996, ISTCS.

[29]  W. Danziger,et al.  Age and the perception of incomplete figures. , 1978, Experimental aging research.

[30]  V. Bruce,et al.  Visual Cognition: Computational, Experimental, and Neuropsychological Perspectives , 1989 .

[31]  T. Valentine,et al.  Naming Faces: The Effects of Facial Distinctiveness and Surname Frequency , 1995 .

[32]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .

[33]  E. Renzi,et al.  VISUAL RECOGNITION IN PATIENTS WITH UNILATERAL CEREBRAL DISEASE , 1966, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[34]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[35]  E. Warrington,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychology: A Clinical Introduction , 1990 .

[36]  B. Rosen,et al.  Category-specific brain activation in fMRI during picture naming. , 1995, Neuroreport.

[37]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[38]  Samuel Howard Bartley Introduction to perception / Samuel Howard Bartley , 1980 .

[39]  Mooney Cm A factorial study of closure. , 1954 .

[40]  Elizabeth K. Warrington,et al.  Disorders of visual perception in patients with localised cerebral lesions , 1967 .

[41]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Functional Brain Imaging Studies of Cortical Mechanisms for Memory , 1995, Science.

[42]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[43]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[44]  Robert T. Wilkinson,et al.  EEG event-related potentials and signal detection , 1978, Biological Psychology.

[45]  J G Snodgrass,et al.  Perceptual Identification Thresholds for 150 Fragmented Pictures from the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Picture Set , 1988, Perceptual and motor skills.

[46]  S. Grossberg The Attentive Brain , 1995 .

[47]  R. Verleger Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3 , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[48]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[49]  D. Stuss,et al.  Perceptual closure and object identification: Electrophysiological responses to incomplete pictures , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[50]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Accuracy of feeling-of-knowing judgments for predicting perceptual identification and relearning. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[51]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[52]  Jeffery W. Elias,et al.  An Analysis of Task Difficulty Using the Visually Degraded Stimulus Task , 1991, Perceptual and motor skills.

[53]  W. Wippich,et al.  Implizites Gedächtnis bei Vorschulkindern : Eine Prozessanalyse beim wiederholten Benennen von Bildern , 1991 .

[54]  E. S. Gollin,et al.  Developmental Studies of Visual Recognition of Incomplete Objects , 1960 .