Cognitive decline in old age: separating retest effects from the effects of growing older.

Repeated administration of cognitive tests improves test performance, making it difficult for researchers to gauge the true extent of age-related cognitive decline. The authors examined this issue using data from the Rush Religious Orders Study and linear mixed-effects models. At annual intervals for up to 8 years, more than 800 older Catholic clergy members completed the same set of 19 cognitive tests from which previously established composite measures of cognitive domains were derived. Retest effects on some measures were substantial and continued to accumulate even after 8 annual test readministrations, but effects on other measures were minimal. Across cognitive measures, retest effects were not related to age, sex, or education. Individual differences in retest effects were substantial but not consistent across cognitive measures. The results suggest that retest-based improvement in cognitive test performance can be substantial and persistent.

[1]  R. Dixon,et al.  Longitudinal changes in quantitative and qualitative indicators of word and story recall in young-old and old-old adults. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[2]  H J Sagar,et al.  Incidental and intentional recall in Parkinson's disease: an account based on diminished attentional resources. , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  H. Akaike A new look at the statistical model identification , 1974 .

[4]  J. Schneider,et al.  Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. , 2002, JAMA.

[5]  D. Bennett,et al.  Religious Orders Study: Overview and Change in Cognitive and Motor Speed , 2004 .

[6]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[7]  J. Morris,et al.  The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assesment of Alzheimer's disease , 1989, Neurology.

[8]  G. Belle,et al.  Cross‐Sectional Versus Longitudinal Estimates of Cognitive Change in Nondemented Older People: A CERAD Study , 1999, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[9]  D. Bennett,et al.  Person-specific paths of cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease and their relation to age. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[10]  K. Liang,et al.  Asymptotic Properties of Maximum Likelihood Estimators and Likelihood Ratio Tests under Nonstandard Conditions , 1987 .

[11]  D. A. Bennett,et al.  Natural history of mild cognitive impairment in older persons , 2002, Neurology.

[12]  John J. McArdle,et al.  Expanding test–retest designs to include developmental time-lag components. , 1997 .

[13]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.

[14]  Emilio Ferrer,et al.  Modeling age and retest processes in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[15]  D Commenges,et al.  A 5-year longitudinal study of the Mini-Mental State Examination in normal aging. , 1997, American journal of epidemiology.

[16]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Improvement or simply practice? The effects of twenty repeated assessments on people with and without brain injury , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[17]  David A. Bennett,et al.  The apolipoprotein E ε2 allele and decline in episodic memory , 2002 .

[18]  C. Morrell,et al.  Likelihood ratio testing of variance components in the linear mixed-effects model using restricted maximum likelihood. , 1998, Biometrics.

[19]  G. Molenberghs,et al.  Linear Mixed Models for Longitudinal Data , 2001 .

[20]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Multivariate modeling of age and retest in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[21]  P. Diggle,et al.  Identifying and separating the effects of practice and of cognitive ageing during a large longitudinal study of elderly community residents , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[23]  P. Diggle,et al.  Practice and drop-out effects during a 17-year longitudinal study of cognitive aging. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[24]  Emilio Ferrer,et al.  Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[25]  P. Satz,et al.  Effect of repeated administration of a neuropsychological battery in the elderly. , 1991, Journal of clinical psychology.

[26]  A. Tröster,et al.  Test-retest stability of the California verbal learning test in older persons. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[27]  I. Grant,et al.  Test–retest reliability and practice effects of Expanded Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery , 1999, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[28]  J. Schneider,et al.  Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[29]  J. Greene Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment , 1995 .

[30]  J. Mcardle,et al.  Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life span. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[31]  J. Ware,et al.  Random-effects models for longitudinal data. , 1982, Biometrics.

[32]  K A Ericcson,et al.  Acquisition of a memory skill. , 1980, Science.

[33]  I. Siegler,et al.  Cross-sectional and longitudinal patterns of three Wechsler Memory Scale Subtests. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[34]  M. Albert,et al.  Use of brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer's disease. , 1991, The International journal of neuroscience.

[35]  D. Hultsch,et al.  Memory Change in the Aged , 1998 .