Phonological reduction in maternal speech in northern Australian English: change over time*

ABSTRACT Segmental variation in maternal speech to children changes over time. This study investigated variation in non-citation speech processes in a longitudinal, 26-hour corpus of maternal northern Australian English. Recordings were naturalistic parent–child interactions when children (N=4) were 1;6, 2;0, and 2;6. The mothers' speech was phonetically transcribed and analyzed. Based on previous sociophonetic research showing proportional changes in speech variants in maternal speech as children get older, it was predicted that deletion of word-initial /h/ and word-final /v/, processes common in non-citation speech, would increase over time. Instead results showed a non-linear change in deletion within a stable set of lexical items. Deletion proportionately increased between 1;6 and 2;0 and decreased between 2;0 and 2;6. Further analysis indicated increased deletion was not accounted for by changes in speech rate, which only marginally increased over time. Findings suggest mothers fine-tune differentially over time as children's receptive and productive language knowledge develops.

[1]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[2]  Björn Lindblom,et al.  Explaining Phonetic Variation: A Sketch of the H&H Theory , 1990 .

[3]  Felicity Cox,et al.  The Border Effect: Vowel Differences across the NSW - Victorian Border ∗ , 2003 .

[4]  Segmental distribution patterns of English infant- and adult-directed speech* , 2009, Journal of Child Language.

[5]  A. G. Mitchell,et al.  The pronunciation of English in Australia , 1946 .

[6]  Heather Buchan Phonetic variation in Gurindji Kriol and northern Australian English: a longitudinal study of fricatives in maternal speech , 2012 .

[7]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. , 1994 .

[8]  P. MacNeilage,et al.  Segmental properties of input to infants: a study of Korean , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[9]  Lori Markson,et al.  Orienting to third-party conversations* , 2010, Journal of Child Language.

[10]  J. Pierrehumbert Phonetic Diversity, Statistical Learning, and Acquisition of Phonology , 2003, Language and speech.

[11]  Paul Foulkes,et al.  The social life of phonetics and phonology , 2006, J. Phonetics.

[12]  John Ingram,et al.  Connected Speech Processes in Australian English. , 1989 .

[13]  E. Bard,et al.  The unintelligibility of speech to children , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  Jean-Pierre Chevrot,et al.  Liaison acquisition, word segmentation and construction in French: a usage-based account , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[15]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Recognition and Representation of Function Words in English-Learning Infants , 2006 .

[16]  John C. Wells,et al.  Accents of English , 1982 .

[17]  V. Youssef Children's linguistic choices: Audience design and societal norms , 1993, Language in Society.

[18]  Janet S. Oh,et al.  Overhearing a Language During Childhood , 2002, Psychological science.

[19]  Felicity Cox,et al.  Broadness variation in Australian English speaking females , 2010 .

[20]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .

[21]  V. Youssef Variation as a feature of language acquisition in the Trinidad context , 1991, Language Variation and Change.

[22]  M. Soderstrom,et al.  Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants , 2007 .

[23]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.

[24]  Jennifer Smith,et al.  “Mam, my trousers is fa'in doon!”: Community, caregiver, and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect , 2007, Language Variation and Change.

[25]  Jacques Koreman,et al.  Perceived speech rate: the effects of articulation rate and speaking style in spontaneous speech. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  P. Foulkes,et al.  Phonological Variation in Child-Directed Speech , 2005 .

[27]  Eon-Suk Ko Nonlinear development of speaking rate in child-directed speech , 2011 .

[28]  Mary E. Beckman,et al.  Phonological Representationsin Language Acquisition: Climbing The Ladder of Abstraction , 2011 .

[29]  P. Kerswill,et al.  Creating a New Town koine: Children and language change in Milton Keynes , 2000, Language in Society.

[30]  Sali A. Tagliamonte,et al.  New perspectives on an ol' variable: (t,d) in British English , 2005, Language Variation and Change.

[31]  J. Pine Input and interaction in language acquisition: The language of primary caregivers , 1994 .

[32]  Barbara M. Horvath,et al.  Variation in Australian English. The Sociolects of Sydney , 1987 .

[33]  Jennifer Smith,et al.  Universal and dialect-specific pathways of acquisition: Caregivers, children, and t/d deletion , 2009, Language Variation and Change.

[34]  K. Rice Principles of Linguistic Change (Volume 2): Social Factors: William Labov , 2002 .

[35]  Nameera Akhtar,et al.  The robustness of learning through overhearing. , 2005, Developmental science.

[36]  Todd Wareham,et al.  Phon 1.2: A Computational Basis for Phonological Database Elaboration and Model Testing , 2007 .

[37]  Felicity Meakins,et al.  Comparing Vowels in Gurindji Kriol and Katherine English: Citation Speech Data , 2011 .

[38]  N. Ratner Patterns of vowel modification in mother–child speech , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[39]  Patricia K Kuhl,et al.  Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study* , 2009, Journal of Child Language.

[40]  A. Marchal,et al.  Speech production and speech modelling , 1990 .

[41]  William Labov,et al.  The child as linguistic historian , 1989, Language Variation and Change.

[42]  K. Stevens,et al.  Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. , 1992, Science.

[43]  P. Foulkes,et al.  South East London English: discrete versus continuous modelling of consonantal reduction , 2014 .

[44]  A. Fernald,et al.  A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[45]  D. Stern,et al.  The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[46]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Sensitivity to the Sound Patterns of Native Language Words , 1993 .

[47]  L. Shockey,et al.  Phonological Processes in Speech Addressed to Children , 1980 .

[48]  A. Cutler Phonological cues to open- and closed-class words in the processing of spoken sentences , 1993, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.

[49]  Alan Cruttenden,et al.  Input and interaction in language acquisition: Phonetic and prosodic aspects of Baby Talk , 1994 .

[50]  S. Barbu,et al.  Developmental dynamics of SES-related differences in children's production of obligatory and variable phonological alternations , 2011 .

[51]  Felicity Meakins,et al.  Case-Marking in Contact: The development and function of case morphology in Gurindji Kriol , 2011 .

[52]  W. Labov Principles Of Linguistic Change , 1994 .

[53]  José Ignacio Hualde,et al.  Illustrations of the IPA , 2012 .

[54]  J. Roberts,et al.  Acquisition of variable rules: a study of (-t, d) deletion in preschool children , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[55]  Nan Bernstein Ratner,et al.  Phonological rule usage in mother-child speech , 1984 .

[56]  J. Harrington,et al.  An acoustic phonetic study of broad, general, and cultivated Australian English vowels* , 1997 .

[57]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. , 1997, Science.

[58]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  The differential role of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorisation , 2005, Cognition.