Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R Parasuraman,et al. Controlling the focus of spatial attention during visual search: effects of advanced aging and Alzheimer disease. , 1997, Neuropsychology.
[2] E. Maylor,et al. Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] R. de Bleser,et al. Visual agnosia: a case of reduced attentional "spotlight"? , 1995, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.
[4] J. Hodges,et al. Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer's disease. A critical review. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[5] M. Goldberg,et al. Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.
[6] K. Heilman,et al. Neglect I: clinical and anatomic issues , 2000 .
[7] M. Petrides,et al. Directed attention after unilateral frontal excisions in humans , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[8] M. Jeannerod,et al. The Directional Coding of Reaching Movements. A Visuomotor Conception of Spatial Neglect , 1987 .
[9] J. Wade,et al. The clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. , 1987, Archives of neurology.
[10] F. J. Friedrich,et al. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[11] H. J. Muller,et al. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] S. Petersen,et al. Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[13] J. Haxby,et al. Attention and brain function in Alzheimer's disease: A review. , 1993 .
[14] M. Perenin,et al. Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.
[15] E. Vogel,et al. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[16] Elisabetta Làdavas,et al. Automatic and voluntary orienting of attention in patients with visual neglect: Horizontal and vertical dimensions , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[17] M. Folstein,et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.
[18] R. Rafal,et al. Visual Extinction and Stimulus Repetition , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[19] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[20] Martha J. Farah,et al. Patient-based approaches to cognitive neuroscience , 2000 .
[21] Martha J. Farah,et al. Mechanisms of Spatial Attention: The Relation of Macrostructure to Microstructure in Parietal Neglect , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[22] A. Allport. Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .
[23] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[24] M. Eckstein. The Lower Visual Search Efficiency for Conjunctions Is Due to Noise and not Serial Attentional Processing , 1998 .
[25] Robert D. Nebes. The cognitive neuropsychology of Alzheimer's disease. , 2000 .
[26] E. Tangalos,et al. CME Practice parameter: , 2022 .
[27] J. Deutsch. Perception and Communication , 1958, Nature.
[28] E. Làdavas,et al. Is the hemispatial deficit produced by right parietal lobe damage associated with retinal or gravitational coordinates? , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[29] J. Driver,et al. Can Visual Neglect Operate in Object-centred Co-ordinates? An Affirmative Single-case Study , 1991 .
[30] Jamshed J. Bharucha,et al. STEVENS’ HANDBOOK OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY , 2002 .
[31] M. Mesulam. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.
[32] R. Nebes,et al. Focused and Divided Attention in Alzheimer's Disease , 1989, Cortex.
[33] M. Farah,et al. Does visual attention select objects or locations? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[34] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[35] Lynn C. Robertson,et al. The neurology of visual attention. , 1995 .
[36] P. Eslinger,et al. Principles and Practice of Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology , 2003 .
[37] Shaun P. Vecera,et al. Grouped locations and object-based attention: Comment on Egly , 1994 .
[38] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Alzheimer disease constricts the dynamic range of spatial attention in visual search , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[39] R. Rafal,et al. Progressive supranuclear palsy , 1981, Neurology.
[40] R. Douglas,et al. Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] H. Coslett,et al. Narrowing the spotlight: A visual attentional disorder in presumed alzheimer's disease , 1995 .
[42] J. Wolfe,et al. Inhibitory tagging in visual search: A failure to replicate , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[43] C. Bundesen. A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.
[44] J. Semmes,et al. Visual Agnosia: A Case of Reduced Attentional “Spotlight”? , 1992, Cortex.
[45] A. Damasio,et al. Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.
[46] M. Posner,et al. Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .
[47] H. Karnath,et al. Trunk orientation as the determining factor of the 'contralateral' deficit in the neglect syndrome and as the physical anchor of the internal representation of body orientation in space. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[48] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[49] Lynn C. Robertson,et al. Visual Search Performance in the Neglect Syndrome , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[50] M. Posner,et al. Neural systems control of spatial orienting. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[51] K Ball,et al. Visual attention impairments in Alzheimer’s disease , 2000, Neurology.
[52] P. Rabbitt,et al. Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption , 1989 .
[53] S. Petersen,et al. Pulvinar nuclei of the behaving rhesus monkey: visual responses and their modulation. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.
[54] M. Farah,et al. Unconscious perception of “extinguished” visual stimuli: Reassessing the evidence , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[55] M. Jeannerod. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .
[56] M. Posner,et al. Does attention affect visual feature integration? , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of visual attention in progressive supranuclear palsy. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[58] P. Bartolomeo,et al. Modulating the attentional bias in unilateral neglect: the effects of the strategic set , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[59] U. Neisser. VISUAL SEARCH. , 1964, Scientific American.
[60] V. Ramachandran,et al. Encyclopedia of the Human Brain , 2002 .
[61] P. Cavanagh,et al. The Spatial Resolution of Visual Attention , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[62] G. Woodman,et al. Voluntary and automatic attentional control of visual working memory , 2002 .
[63] D. Stuss,et al. Visual attention deficits in Alzheimer's disease: simple versus conjoined feature search. , 1999, Neuropsychology.
[64] R. Rafal,et al. Attention and Feature Integration: Illusory Conjunctions in a Patient with a Parietal Lobe Lesion , 1991 .
[65] D. Spalding. The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.
[66] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness , 1996, Nature.
[67] [Neuropsychology of mental imagery]. , 1982, L'annee psychologique.
[68] P. Carpenter,et al. Frames of reference for allocating attention to space: Evidence from the neglect syndrome , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[69] M. Posner,et al. Deficits in human visual spatial attention following thalamic lesions. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[70] Matthew Rizzo,et al. Visual search in hemineglect : what stirs idle eyes? , 1992 .
[71] P. Bartolomeo,et al. Position of the Egocentric Reference and Directional Arm Movements in Right-Brain-Damaged Patients , 1998, Brain and Cognition.
[72] C. L. M.. The Psychology of Attention , 1890, Nature.
[73] H. Karnath,et al. Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[74] K. Heilman,et al. Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.
[75] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[76] E. Bisiach,et al. Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.
[77] K. Cave,et al. Spatial Attention in Visual Search for Features and Feature Conjunctions , 1995 .
[78] J. Jonides. Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .
[79] J. Jonides,et al. Spatial working memory and spatial selective attention. , 1998 .
[80] A. Treisman. Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[81] Alessandro Farnè,et al. In search of biased egocentric reference frames in neglect , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[82] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Genetics and visual attention: Selective deficits in healthy adult carriers of the ɛ4 allele of the apolipoprotein E gene , 2000 .
[83] Shaun P. Vecera,et al. Toward a Biased Competition Account of Object-Based Segregation and Attention , 2000 .
[84] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[85] A. Treisman,et al. Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[86] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and performance IX , 1982 .
[87] K M Heilman,et al. Performance On hemispatial pointing task by patients with neglect syndrome , 1983, Neurology.
[88] J. Moran,et al. Sensation and perception , 1980 .
[89] A. Treisman,et al. Parietal contributions to visual feature binding: evidence from a patient with bilateral lesions , 1995, Science.
[90] J. P. Thomas,et al. A signal detection model predicts the effects of set size on visual search accuracy for feature, conjunction, triple conjunction, and disjunction displays , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[91] J. Palmer. Attention in Visual Search: Distinguishing Four Causes of a Set-Size Effect , 1995 .
[92] B. Dosher,et al. External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms , 1998, Vision Research.
[93] Jordan Grafman,et al. Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .
[94] D. Levine,et al. Left visual spatial neglect is both environment‐centered and body‐centered , 1987, Neurology.
[95] S. Ferber,et al. Parietal and occipital lobe contributions to perception of straight ahead orientation , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[96] A. Treisman. The binding problem , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[97] M. Farah,et al. Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: Modality-specific or supramodal? , 1989, Neuropsychologia.
[98] M M Mesulam,et al. Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.
[99] J V Haxby,et al. Visuospatial attention in dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[100] F. J. Friedrich,et al. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of visual attention , 1984 .
[101] H. Pashler,et al. Negligible Effect of Spatial Precuing on Identification of Single Digits , 1994 .
[102] K M Heilman,et al. Bodily neglect and orientational biases in unilateral neglect syndrome and normal subjects , 1990, Neurology.
[103] A. Burns. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1991 .
[104] Raymond Klein,et al. Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search , 1988, Nature.
[105] J. Driver,et al. Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[106] J. Driver,et al. Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[107] R. Parasuraman. The attentive brain , 1998 .
[108] M. Posner,et al. The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.
[109] Avishai Henik,et al. Endogenously Generated and Visually Guided Saccades after Lesions of the Human Frontal Eye Fields , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[110] D. Balota,et al. Inhibition of return and visuospatial attention in healthy older adults and individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1997, Neuropsychology.
[111] S. Vecera,et al. Psychoanatomical substrates of Bálint's syndrome , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[112] Christof Zoelch,et al. Disorders of visuospatial orientation in the frontal plane in patients with visual neglect following right or left parietal lesions , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[113] Attention-based rehearsal in spatial working memory. , 1996 .
[114] Geoffrey F Woodman,et al. Perceptual organization influences visual working memory , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[115] R. Klein,et al. Inhibition of Return is a Foraging Facilitator in Visual Search , 1999 .
[116] M. Jeannerod,et al. In search for the egocentric reference. A neurophysiological hypothesis , 1984, Neuropsychologia.
[117] B. Milner,et al. Effects of frontal or temporal lobectomy on the use of advance information in a choice reaction time task , 1989, Neuropsychologia.