Orientation sensitivity to graspable objects: An fMRI adaptation study

It has been proposed that vision-for-perception and vision-for-action are subserved by distinct streams of visual processing, the ventral and dorsal stream, respectively [Milner, A. D., Goodale, M. A., 1995. The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, Oxford]. Such a distinction has been supported by a recent functional magnetic resonance (fMR) adaptation study [Valyear, K. F., Culham, J. C., Sharif, N., Westwood, D., Goodale, M. A., 2006. A double dissociation between sensitivity to changes in object identity and object orientation in the ventral and dorsal visual streams: A human fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 44, 218-228], which demonstrated selectivity to object identity but not object orientation within the ventral stream, and selectivity to object orientation but not object identity within the dorsal stream. These results were interpreted as suggesting that changes to object identity (but not to orientation) would alter the representation of the stimulus in the perceptual/recognition system, whereas changes in object orientation (but not necessarily identity) would alter the coding of the stimulus within a visuomotor system concerned with behaviour such as grasping. If orientation sensitivity in the dorsal stream does reflect such a potential for action, then this sensitivity should be specific to graspable objects. Using an fMR adaptation paradigm, we presented participants with an image of either a graspable or non-graspable stimulus, followed by the same image in either the original orientation or its mirror image. One region within the dorsal stream, the lateral occipito-parietal junction (lOPJ), was shown to be sensitive to orientation changes for graspable stimuli; this region did not show orientation sensitivity for non-graspable stimuli. Thus, it appears that the sensitivity to orientation changes in this region is specific to graspable objects, presumably because such changes affect the affordances of graspable but not non-graspable objects.

[1]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  The Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus Mediates Grasp Execution, Independent of Requirement to Update: New Insights from Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  H. Sakata,et al.  The TINS Lecture The parietal association cortex in depth perception and visual control of hand action , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[4]  Tomoka Naganuma,et al.  Neural Correlates for Perception of 3D Surface Orientation from Texture Gradient , 2002, Science.

[5]  H. Sakata,et al.  Parietal neurons represent surface orientation from the gradient of binocular disparity. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  T. R. Jordan,et al.  Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  H. Sakata,et al.  Neural mechanisms of visual guidance of hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  E. Wojciulik,et al.  Attention increases neural selectivity in the human lateral occipital complex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  K. Grill-Spector The neural basis of object perception , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[11]  R. Malach,et al.  The topography of high-order human object areas , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  C. Büchel,et al.  Surface orientation discrimination activates caudal and anterior intraparietal sulcus in humans: an event-related fMRI study. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Intact automatic avoidance of obstacles in patients with visual form agnosia , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Rafael Malach,et al.  Large-Scale Mirror-Symmetry Organization of Human Occipito-Temporal Object Areas , 2003, Neuron.

[16]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[17]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  A parieto-premotor network for object manipulation: evidence from neuroimaging , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  A Parametric fMRI Study of Overt and Covert Shifts of Visuospatial Attention , 2001, NeuroImage.

[19]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Human Brain Activity Related to the Perception of Spatial Features of Objects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[20]  A. Milner,et al.  Automatic avoidance of obstacles is a dorsal stream function: evidence from optic ataxia , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Goodale,et al.  Sight Unseen: An Exploration of Conscious and Unconscious Vision , 2004 .

[22]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[24]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[25]  M. Goodale,et al.  An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[26]  M. Taira,et al.  Cortical Areas Related to Attention to 3D Surface Structures Based on Shading: An fMRI Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[27]  K Tsutsui,et al.  Neural coding of 3D features of objects for hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[28]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Daniel C. Javitt,et al.  Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis☆ , 2003, NeuroImage.

[30]  M. Goodale,et al.  Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography , 1994, Nature.

[32]  Kenneth F. Valyear,et al.  A double dissociation between sensitivity to changes in object identity and object orientation in the ventral and dorsal visual streams: A human fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  H. Sakata,et al.  Neural representation of three-dimensional features of manipulation objects with stereopsis , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[35]  M. Perenin,et al.  Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  G A Orban,et al.  Orientation discrimination of objects and gratings compared: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Cortical topography of human anterior intraparietal cortex active during visually guided grasping. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[38]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Center–periphery organization of human object areas , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[40]  Volkmar Glauche,et al.  Functional properties and interaction of the anterior and posterior intraparietal areas in humans , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[41]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Visual pathways for object-oriented action and object recognition: functional anatomy with PET. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension , 1994, Current Biology.

[44]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  R. Passingham,et al.  Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.