Predictive eye movements in natural vision

In the natural world, the brain must handle inherent delays in visual processing. This is a problem particularly during dynamic tasks. A possible solution to visuo-motor delays is prediction of a future state of the environment based on the current state and properties of the environment learned from experience. Prediction is well known to occur in both saccades and pursuit movements and is likely to depend on some kind of internal visual model as the basis for this prediction. However, most evidence comes from controlled laboratory studies using simple paradigms. In this study, we examine eye movements made in the context of demanding natural behavior, while playing squash. We show that prediction is a pervasive component of gaze behavior in this context. We show in addition that these predictive movements are extraordinarily precise and operate continuously in time across multiple trajectories and multiple movements. This suggests that prediction is based on complex dynamic visual models of the way that balls move, accumulated over extensive experience. Since eye, head, arm, and body movements all co-occur, it seems likely that a common internal model of predicted visual state is shared by different effectors to allow flexible coordination patterns. It is generally agreed that internal models are responsible for predicting future sensory state for control of body movements. The present work suggests that model-based prediction is likely to be a pervasive component in natural gaze control as well.

[1]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—IV. Anticipatory smooth eye movements depend on prior target motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[2]  J Fukushima,et al.  Predictive signals in the pursuit area of the monkey frontal eye fields. , 2008, Progress in brain research.

[3]  Mary M Hayhoe,et al.  Spatial memory and saccadic targeting in a natural task. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[4]  J. Kohno,et al.  Visualization of the tidal flow in Sasebo and Oomura Bays , 2002 .

[5]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Visuo-motor coordination and internal models for object interception , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Combining Priors and Noisy Visual Cues in a Rapid Pointing Task , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[8]  D. Wolpert Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control. , 2007, Human movement science.

[9]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Paul R. Schrater,et al.  Auxiliary object knowledge influences visually-guided interception behavior , 2005, APGV '05.

[11]  M. Land Eye Movements in Daily Life , 2003 .

[12]  Thomas Nyffeler,et al.  Involvement of the Supplementary Eye Field in Oculomotor Predictive Behavior , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[14]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Pursuit and saccadic tracking exhibit a similar dependence on movement preparation time , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  Adaptive Gaze Control in Natural Environments , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  L. Simo,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of anticipatory behavior: dissociation between sensory-driven and memory-driven systems. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.

[19]  Vincent P. Ferrera,et al.  Estimating invisible target speed from neuronal activity in monkey frontal eye field , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  A. Bahill,et al.  Why can't batters keep their eyes on the ball? , 1984 .

[21]  Kenji Kawano,et al.  Trial-by-trial updating of the gain in preparation for smooth pursuit eye movement based on past experience in humans. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Roland S. Johansson,et al.  Sensory Control of Dexterous Manipulation in Humans , 1996 .

[23]  Peter W Battaglia,et al.  Humans Trade Off Viewing Time and Movement Duration to Improve Visuomotor Accuracy in a Fast Reaching Task , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Neuronal correlates of inferred motion in primate posterior parietal cortex , 1995, Nature.

[26]  Katsumi Aoki,et al.  Recent development of flow visualization , 2004, J. Vis..

[27]  R. Krauzlis The Control of Voluntary Eye Movements: New Perspectives , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[28]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Saccades exhibit abrupt transition between reactive and predictive; predictive saccade sequences have long-term correlations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Kikuro Fukushima,et al.  Eye-Pursuit and Reafferent Head Movement Signals Carried by Pursuit Neurons in the Caudal Part of the Frontal Eye Fields during Head-Free Pursuit , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control , 2022 .

[31]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  The role of internal models and prediction in catching balls , 2005, AAAI 2005.

[32]  J. Saunders,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Joan López-Moliner,et al.  Interceptive timing: prior knowledge matters. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[34]  A. G. Lasker,et al.  Cognitive influences on predictive saccadic tracking , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Effects of learning on smooth pursuit during transient disappearance of a visual target. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Dana H. Ballard,et al.  Modeling embodied visual behaviors , 2007, TAP.

[37]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  G R Barnes,et al.  Evidence for a link between the extra-retinal component of random-onset pursuit and the anticipatory pursuit of predictable object motion. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models and prediction of visual gravitational motion , 2008, Vision Research.

[40]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Evidence for object permanence in the smooth-pursuit eye movements of monkeys. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Vincent P Ferrera,et al.  Modulation of visual responses in macaque frontal eye field during covert tracking of invisible targets. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Wilsaan M. Joiner,et al.  Adaptive Control of Saccades via Internal Feedback , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Hand and brain : the neurophysiology and psychology of hand movements , 1996 .

[44]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  The Role of the Human Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Ocular Motor Behavior , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[45]  John M. Findlay,et al.  Spatial and temporal factors in the predictive generation of saccadic eye movements , 1981, Vision Research.

[46]  Jeff B. Pelz,et al.  Building a lightweight eyetracking headgear , 2004, ETRA.

[47]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Prediction in the oculomotor system: smooth pursuit during transient disappearance of a visual target , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  J. Tresilian,et al.  Systematic Variation in Performance of an Interceptive Action with Changes in the Temporal Constraints , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[49]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye movements in natural behavior , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  D. Knill,et al.  The role of memory in visually guided reaching. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[51]  A. Terry Bahill,et al.  Predicting a Baseball’s Path , 2005 .

[52]  Vincent P Ferrera,et al.  Internally Generated Error Signals in Monkey Frontal Eye Field during an Inferred Motion Task , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  Paul R. Schrater,et al.  Effects of visual uncertainty on grasping movements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[55]  Kikuro Fukushima,et al.  Memory and Decision Making in the Frontal Cortex during Visual Motion Processing for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 2009, Neuron.

[56]  B. Abernethy Expertise, Visual Search, and Information Pick-up in Squash , 1990, Perception.

[57]  M F Land,et al.  The knowledge base of the oculomotor system. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[58]  Michael F. Land,et al.  From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[59]  G. Barnes,et al.  Independent control of head and gaze movements during head‐free pursuit in humans , 1999, The Journal of physiology.