Allocentric visual cues influence mental transformation of bodies.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. Bockisch,et al. Differential Effects of Visual Feedback on Subjective Visual Vertical Accuracy and Precision , 2012, PloS one.
[2] W. Prinz,et al. The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .
[3] Marjolein P. M. Kammers,et al. What is embodiment? A psychometric approach , 2008, Cognition.
[4] R. Shepard,et al. Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.
[5] I P Howard,et al. Visually-induced reorientation illusions as a function of age. , 2000, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[6] Ian P. Howard,et al. Human visual orientation , 1982 .
[7] L. Buxbaum,et al. The Role of the Dynamic Body Schema in Praxis: Evidence from Primary Progressive Apraxia , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[8] Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al. Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations , 2007, NeuroImage.
[9] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.
[10] Catherine L. Reed,et al. What is the body schema , 2002 .
[11] L M Parsons,et al. Imagined spatial transformation of one's body. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[12] D R Proffitt,et al. The influence of spatial reference frames on imagined object- and viewer rotations. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[13] Barbara Tversky,et al. Mental spatial transformations of objects and perspective , 2001, Spatial Cogn. Comput..
[14] M. Kozhevnikov,et al. Perspective-Taking vs. Mental Rotation Transformations and How They Predict Spatial Navigation Performance. , 2006 .
[15] Brice Isableu,et al. Embodied spatial transformations: "body analogy" for the mental rotation of objects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[16] F. Mast,et al. Mental transformation abilities in patients with unilateral and bilateral vestibular loss , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Mary Hegarty,et al. Modulation of neural activity by angle of rotation during imagined spatial transformations , 2006, NeuroImage.
[18] F. Mast,et al. Imagined paralysis impairs embodied spatial transformations , 2011, Cognitive neuroscience.
[19] I. Howard,et al. Visually Induced Reorientation Illusions , 2001, Perception.
[20] L. Harris,et al. How different types of scenes affect the Subjective Visual Vertical (SVV) and the Perceptual Upright (PU) , 2010, Vision Research.
[21] O. Blanke,et al. Differential influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet in left and right handers , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] A. Borghi,et al. Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and sensing the body , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[23] O. Blanke,et al. Neural Basis of Embodiment: Distinct Contributions of Temporoparietal Junction and Extrastriate Body Area , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] M. Hegarty,et al. A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability , 2001, Memory & cognition.
[25] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Two kinds of visual perspective taking , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] F. Mast,et al. Balancing the mind: vestibular induced facilitation of egocentric mental transformations. , 2012, Experimental psychology.
[27] Klaus Kessler,et al. The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: Embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference , 2010, Cognition.
[28] I. Howard,et al. Effect of Field Size, Head Motion, and Rotational Velocity on Roll Vection and Illusory Self-Tilt in a Tumbling Room , 1999, Perception.
[29] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Transformations of visuospatial images. , 2005, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.
[30] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Frames of reference for biological motion and face perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[31] Á. Pascual-Leone,et al. Linking Out-of-Body Experience and Self Processing to Mental Own-Body Imagery at the Temporoparietal Junction , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.