Horace Barlow, doyen of the visual sciences, died on July 5, 2020 at the age of 98. His research illuminated many fundamental issues in vision and his efficient coding hypothesis was a beacon for vision research. His probing mind and playful spirit influenced the questions asked by a generation of vision researchers, bringing us closer to understanding how we see. As a student, Barlow was on track for a medical degree. Through good fortune and research studentships from the Rockefeller Foundation and the Medical Research Council, his talents for scientific inquiry became apparent. His career blossomed in Cambridge after 1950 and he later moved to UC Berkeley in 1964, where, along with Gerald Westheimer and Bill Levick, he attracted and trained many of today’s leading visual physiologists and psychophysicists. He moved back to Cambridge in 1973, where his insights into the neural basis of visual perception deepened. Early in his career, Barlow produced a series of singleauthored landmark studies that are in textbooks. One study from that period asked the question: How sensitive is the retina to detecting photons at absolute threshold (Barlow 1956)? Based on elegant experimental design and careful analysis he concluded that the absorption of a single photon could excite a rod photoreceptor in the human retina. However, because of spontaneous isomerization of rhodopsin and other sources of noise, the coincidence of events in multiple excited rods are needed to evoke the sensation of a light flash. At Berkeley, Barlow discovered neurons in the primary visual cortex tuned for stereoscopic vision. Hubel and Wiesel had reported neurons that receive visual input from both eyes. Barlow found that the best responses occurred not when the visual stimulus was at the same angle from the fovea in the two eyes, but at an offset, or binocular disparity (Barlow et al. 1967). Different cells were tuned to different disparities, corresponding to different depth planes. Barlow was influenced early in his career by Norbert Wiener’s book on Cybernetics (1948) and Claude Shannon’s article on A Mathematical Theory of Communication (1948), which later inspired his insights into neural coding. He was a founder of the Ratio Club in 1949, focused on cybernetics, whose members were an eclectic group of young neurobiologists, engineers, mathematicians and physicists, including Alan Turing, many of whom went on to highly prominent careers. The central tenet of Barlow’s efficient coding hypothesis (Barlow 1959, 1961) was that brains represent information in the world by a minimum number of spikes. His hypothesis that the output of the retina reduced redundancy in visual input provided an elegant explanation for why the receptive fields of ganglion cells had centers and surrounds of opposite polarity, which minimized output spikes for uniformly illuminated visual patches. Barlow further hypothesized that the simple cells discovered by Hubel and Wiesel in the visual cortex, which resembled Gabor functions, were a sparse and efficient representation for natural scenes (Barlow 1983). At the time, there was no way to confirm his hypothesis, but the development of computational methods for sparse coding (Olshausen and Field 1996) and Independent Component Analysis (Bell and Sejnowski 1997) showed that he was right. Localized Gabor functions are a sparse and efficient basis for natural scenes, but not for unnatural images like this printed page. In another influential paper (Barlow 1972) entitled Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology?, Barlow applied his ideas to how neurons represent complex objects at higher levels of the visual cortex, where neural responses are more selective and activity in the neural population sparser than at the early stages. He argued against “pontifical cells,” single neurons that code the percept of unique objects, which was the basis for the so-called “grandmother cell theory” that had taken hold in the vision community. He argued instead that an object should be represented * Terrence J. Sejnowski terry@snl.salk.edu
[1]
Ha Hong,et al.
Performance-optimized hierarchical models predict neural responses in higher visual cortex
,
2014,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[2]
C. Blakemore,et al.
The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination
,
1967,
The Journal of physiology.
[3]
Terrence J Sejnowski,et al.
The unreasonable effectiveness of deep learning in artificial intelligence
,
2020,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[4]
Sang Joon Kim,et al.
A Mathematical Theory of Communication
,
2006
.
[5]
David J. Field,et al.
Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images
,
1996,
Nature.
[6]
H. Barlow.
Retinal noise and absolute threshold.
,
1956,
Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[7]
K. Clark.
CONDITIONS FOR VERSATILE LEARNING , HELMHOLTZ ’ S UNCONSCIOUS INFERENCE , AND THE TASK OF PERCEPTION
,
2002
.
[8]
H. Barlow,et al.
Single Units and Sensation: A Neuron Doctrine for Perceptual Psychology?
,
1972,
Perception.
[9]
Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.
The “independent components” of natural scenes are edge filters
,
1997,
Vision Research.
[10]
H. Barlow.
Conditions for versatile learning, Helmholtz's unconscious inference, and the task of perception
,
1990,
Vision Research.
[11]
Horace Barlow,et al.
Understanding Natural Vision
,
1983
.
[12]
Claude E. Shannon,et al.
A mathematical theory of communication
,
1948,
MOCO.
[13]
P. O. Bishop.
NEURAL MECHANISMS FOR BINOCULAR DEPTH DISCRIMINATION
,
1981
.
[14]
H. B. Barlow,et al.
Possible Principles Underlying the Transformations of Sensory Messages
,
2012
.
[15]
H. Barlow.
Cerebral Cortex as Model Builder
,
1987
.