Lifespan Mental Activity Predicts Diminished Rate of Hippocampal Atrophy

Objective Epidemiological studies suggest that complex mental activity may reduce the risk for dementia, however an underlying mechanism remains unclear. Our objective was to determine whether individual differences in lifespan complex mental activity are linked to altered rates of hippocampal atrophy independent of global measures of neurodegeneration. Methods Thirty seven healthy older individuals had their complex mental activity levels estimated using the Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ) and completed serial MRI investigations at baseline and three years follow-up. Hippocampal volume and semi-automatic quantitation of whole brain volume (WBV) and white matter hyperintensities (WMHs) were compared at both time points. Results Higher LEQ scores were correlated with hippocampal volume independent of covariates at the three year follow-up stage (r = 0.43, p = 0.012). Moreover, those with higher LEQ scores experienced less hippocampal atrophy over the follow-up period (r = 0.41, p = 0.02). High LEQ individuals had less than half the hippocampal volume decline of low LEQ individuals in a multivariate analysis (F = 4.47, p = 0.042). No parallel changes were found in measures of WBV and WMHs. Conclusions High level of complex mental activity across the lifespan was correlated with a reduced rate of hippocampal atrophy. This finding could not be explained by general differences in intracranial volume, larger hippocampi at baseline, presence of hypertensive disease, gender or low mood. Our results suggest that neuroprotection in medial temporal lobe may be one mechanism underlying the link between mental activity and lower rates of dementia observed in population-based studies. Additional studies are required to further explore this novel finding.

[1]  P. Sachdev,et al.  Can cognitive training prevent the onset of dementia? A systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow up , 2009, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[2]  Perminder Sachdev,et al.  Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of dementia? Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow-up. , 2009, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[3]  Michael J Valenzuela,et al.  Brain reserve and the prevention of dementia , 2008, Current opinion in psychiatry.

[4]  M. Breakspear,et al.  Complex mental activity and the aging brain: Molecular, cellular and cortical network mechanisms , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[5]  H. Potter,et al.  Enhanced cognitive activity—over and above social or physical activity—is required to protect Alzheimer’s mice against cognitive impairment, reduce Aβ deposition, and increase synaptic immunoreactivity , 2007, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[6]  P. Sachdev,et al.  Assessment of complex mental activity across the lifespan: development of the Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ) , 2006, Psychological Medicine.

[7]  A. Hannan,et al.  Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  P. Sachdev,et al.  Brain reserve and cognitive decline: a non-parametric systematic review , 2006, Psychological Medicine.

[9]  P. Sachdev,et al.  Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review , 2005, Psychological Medicine.

[10]  N. Okado,et al.  Modification of AMPA receptor properties following environmental enrichment , 2005, Brain and Development.

[11]  D. Borchelt,et al.  Environmental Enrichment Mitigates Cognitive Deficits in a Mouse Model of Alzheimer's Disease , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Victoria M Perreau,et al.  Voluntary Exercise Decreases Amyloid Load in a Transgenic Model of Alzheimer's Disease , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Ilana S. Hairston,et al.  Environmental Enrichment Reduces Aβ Levels and Amyloid Deposition in Transgenic Mice , 2005, Cell.

[14]  Kaarin J Anstey,et al.  Hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in relation to handedness in adults aged 60 – 64 , 2004, Neuroreport.

[15]  H. Potter,et al.  Environmental enrichment improves cognition in aged Alzheimer's transgenic mice despite stable &bgr;-amyloid deposition , 2004, Neuroreport.

[16]  Wei Wen,et al.  The topography of white matter hyperintensities on brain MRI in healthy 60- to 64-year-old individuals , 2004, NeuroImage.

[17]  J. Nithianantharajah,et al.  Environmental enrichment results in cortical and subcortical changes in levels of synaptophysin and PSD-95 proteins , 2004, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[18]  H. Brodaty,et al.  The neuropsychological profile of vascular cognitive impairment in stroke and TIA patients , 2004, Neurology.

[19]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Environmental Enrichment Rescues Protein Deficits in a Mouse Model of Huntington's Disease, Indicating a Possible Disease Mechanism , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Bogdan Draganski,et al.  Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training , 2004, Nature.

[21]  Timothy C. Bates,et al.  Oral creatine monohydrate supplementation improves brain performance: a double–blind, placebo–controlled, cross–over trial , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  Michael J. Valenzuela,et al.  Memory training alters hippocampal neurochemistry in healthy elderly , 2003, Neuroreport.

[23]  J. Coyle,et al.  Use it or lose it--do effortful mental activities protect against dementia? , 2003, The New England journal of medicine.

[24]  W. Markesbery,et al.  Hippocampal volume as an index of Alzheimer neuropathology: Findings from the Nun Study , 2002, Neurology.

[25]  J. Trollor,et al.  Brain Ageing in the New Millennium , 2001, The Australian and New Zealand journal of psychiatry.

[26]  J. Dubinsky,et al.  On the mechanisms of neuroprotection by creatine and phosphocreatine , 2001, Journal of neurochemistry.

[27]  A. Granholm,et al.  Long-Term Environmental Enrichment Leads to Regional Increases in Neurotrophin Levels in Rat Brain , 2000, Experimental Neurology.

[28]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  A. Mohammed,et al.  Changes in brain nerve growth factor levels and nerve growth factor receptors in rats exposed to environmental enrichment for one year , 1999, Neuroscience.

[30]  C. Jack,et al.  Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment , 1999, Neurology.

[31]  C R Jack,et al.  Volumetric magnetic resonance imaging. Clinical applications and contributions to the understanding of temporal lobe epilepsy. , 1997, Archives of neurology.

[32]  E. Nestler,et al.  A molecular and cellular theory of depression. , 1997, Archives of general psychiatry.

[33]  D R Wekstein,et al.  Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life. Findings from the Nun Study. , 1996, JAMA.

[34]  W. Greenough,et al.  Exercise and the Brain: Angiogenesis in the Adult Rat Cerebellum after Vigorous Physical Activity and Motor Skill Learning , 1992, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[35]  F. Gage,et al.  Adult neurogenesis , 2007, Scholarpedia.

[36]  G. Kempermann,et al.  Lifespan Development and the Brain: Adult Neurogenesis , 2006 .

[37]  H. Braak,et al.  Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes , 2004, Acta Neuropathologica.

[38]  N. Raz Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. , 2000 .