The assessment of non visual maritime cognitive maps of a blind sailor: a case study

Abstract Please click here to download the map associated with this article. Nowadays, thanks to the accessibility of GPS, sighted people widely use electronic charts to navigate through different kinds of environments. In the maritime domain, it has considerably improved the precision of course control. In this domain, blind sailors can not make a compass bearing, however they are able to interact with multimodal electronic charts. Indeed, we conceived SeaTouch, a haptic (tactilekinesthetic) and auditory virtual environment that allows users to perform virtual maritime navigation without vision. In this study we attempt to assess if heading or northing “haptic” views during virtual navigation training influences non-visual spatial knowledge. After simulating a navigation session in each condition, a blind sailor truly navigated on the sea and estimated seamark bearings. We used the triangulation technique to compare the efficiency of northing and heading virtual training. The results are congruent with current knowledge about spatial frames of reference and suggest that getting lost in heading mode forces the blind sailor to coordinate his current “view” with a more global and stable representation.

[1]  Megan M. Lawrence,et al.  A Haptic Soundscape Map of the University of Oregon , 2009 .

[2]  Gunnar Jansson,et al.  The PHANToM used without visual guidance. , 1999 .

[3]  Dan Jacobson Haptic Soundscapes: Developing Novel Multi-Sensory Tools to Promote Access to Geographic Information , 2004 .

[4]  Simon Ungar,et al.  Cognitive mapping without visual experience , 2018, Cognitive Mapping.

[5]  Martin J. Farrell,et al.  Mental Rotation and the Automatic Updating of Body-Centered Spatial Relationships , 1998 .

[6]  Simon Ungar,et al.  Visually impaired children's strategies for memorising a map , 1995 .

[7]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense? , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  E. Spelke,et al.  Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .

[9]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  Florence Gaunet,et al.  Early-Blind Subjects' Spatial Representation of Manipulatory Space: Exploratory Strategies and Reaction to Change , 1997, Perception.

[11]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Representations: Definitions, Distinctions, and Interconnections , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[12]  Hui Zhang,et al.  Novel-view scene recognition relies on identifying spatial reference directions , 2009, Cognition.

[13]  Kirsten Rassmus-Gröhn,et al.  A dynamic haptic-audio traffic environment , 2004 .

[14]  David Mioduser,et al.  Haptic-feedback support for cognitive mapping of unknown spaces by people who are blind , 2008, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[15]  Thomas Porathe,et al.  Measuring effective map design for route guidance: an experiment comparing electronic map display principles , 2007 .

[16]  C. Wickens Human Factors in Vector Map Design: The Importance of Task-Display Dependence , 2000, Journal of Navigation.

[17]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Early-Blind Subjects' Spatial Abilities in the Locomotor Space: Exploratory Strategies and Reaction-to-Change Performance , 1996, Perception.

[18]  J. M. Tellevik Influence of Spatial Exploration Patterns on Cognitive Mapping by Blindfolded Sighted Persons , 1992 .

[19]  H L Pick,et al.  The content and manipulation of cognitive maps in children and adults. , 1976, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[20]  R. Kitchin,et al.  Techniques to Collect and Analyze the Cognitive Map Knowledge of Persons with Visual Impairment Or Blindness: Issues of Validity , 1997 .

[21]  J. Fletcher Spatial Representation in Blind Children. 1: Development Compared to Sighted Children , 1980 .