Using the Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) System to Investigate Cultural Differences in Conversational Turn Count.

Purpose This study investigates how the variables of culture and hearing status might influence the amount of parent-child talk families engage in throughout an average day. Method Seventeen Vietnamese and 8 Canadian families of children with hearing loss and 17 Vietnamese and 13 Canadian families with typically hearing children between the ages of 18 and 48 months old participated in this cross-comparison design study. Each child wore a Language ENvironment Analysis system digital language processor for 3 days. An automated vocal analysis then calculated an average conversational turn count (CTC) for each participant as the variable of investigation. The CTCs for the 4 groups were compared using a Kruskal-Wallis test and a set of planned pairwise comparisons. Results The Canadian families participated in significantly more conversational turns than the Vietnamese families. No significant difference was found between the Vietnamese or the Canadian cohorts as a function of hearing status. Conclusions Culture, but not hearing status, influences CTCs as derived by the Language ENvironment Analysis system. Clinicians should consider how cultural communication practices might influence their suggestions for language stimulation.

[1]  M. Rowe,et al.  Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[2]  Tran Hien Nguyen,et al.  Congenital rubella syndrome (CRS) in Vietnam 2011-2012--CRS epidemic after rubella epidemic in 2010-2011. , 2015, Vaccine.

[3]  P. Hepper,et al.  Development of fetal hearing , 1994, Archives of disease in childhood. Fetal and neonatal edition.

[4]  D. Suskind,et al.  Pilot testing of a parent-directed intervention (Project ASPIRE) for underserved children who are deaf or hard of hearing , 2014 .

[5]  P. Miller,et al.  Self‐Construction through Narrative Practices: A Chinese and American Comparison of Early Socialization , 1996 .

[6]  M. Rowe A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. , 2012, Child development.

[7]  Marc W. Hernandez,et al.  Project ASPIRE: Spoken Language Intervention Curriculum for Parents of Low-socioeconomic Status and Their Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children , 2016, Otology & neurotology : official publication of the American Otological Society, American Neurotology Society [and] European Academy of Otology and Neurotology.

[8]  M. Atherton,et al.  The World Report on Disability in relation to the development of speech-language pathology in Viet Nam , 2013, International journal of speech-language pathology.

[9]  William G. Cochran,et al.  Experimental Designs, 2nd Edition , 1950 .

[10]  J. Gilkerson,et al.  Effects of Quantitative Linguistic Feedback to Caregivers of Young Children , 2015 .

[11]  J. Johnston,et al.  Cross-cultural differences in beliefs and practices that affect the language spoken to children: mothers with Indian and Western heritage. , 2007, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[12]  Catherine E. Snow,et al.  Input and interaction in language acquisition: Beginning from Baby Talk: twenty years of research on input in interaction , 1994 .

[13]  R. Mitchell,et al.  Chasing the Mythical Ten Percent: Parental Hearing Status of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students in the United States , 2004 .

[14]  R. Briggs,et al.  Long-term Communication Outcomes for Children Receiving Cochlear Implants Younger Than 12 Months: A Multicenter Study , 2016, Otology & neurotology : official publication of the American Otological Society, American Neurotology Society [and] European Academy of Otology and Neurotology.

[15]  Jin Y. Shin,et al.  The effects of a home-based intervention conducted by college students for young children with developmental delays in Vietnam , 2016, International journal of developmental disabilities.

[16]  E. Hoff The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. , 2003, Child development.

[17]  C. E. Mobley,et al.  The effect of hearing impairment on the quality of attachment and mother-toddler interaction. , 1990, Child development.

[18]  S. Luksaneeyanawin,et al.  Language Socialization of the Child through Caretaker-Child Personal Narratives , 2006 .

[19]  Ruth K. Chao,et al.  Chinese and European American cultural models of the self reflected in mothers' childrearing beliefs. , 1995 .

[20]  H. Le Opening the Gates for Children with Disabilities An Introduction to Inclusive Education in Vietnam , 2013 .

[21]  M. Soderstrom,et al.  When Do Caregivers Talk? The Influences of Activity and Time of Day on Caregiver Speech and Child Vocalizations in Two Childcare Environments , 2013, PloS one.

[22]  Yiyuan Xu,et al.  Mainland Chinese parenting styles and parent–child interaction , 2005 .

[23]  Mary Pat Moeller,et al.  Linguistic Input, Electronic Media, and Communication Outcomes of Toddlers With Hearing Loss , 2014, Ear and hearing.

[24]  Mary Pat Moeller,et al.  Quantity of parental language in the home environments of hard-of-hearing 2-year-olds. , 2012, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.

[25]  R. Brown,et al.  A First Language , 1973 .

[26]  Keith J. Topping,et al.  Effects of feedback on parent–child language with infants and toddlers in Korea , 2016 .

[27]  Alison J. Bahnsen,et al.  Predictors of morphosyntactic growth in typically developing toddlers: contributions of parent input and child sex. , 2011, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[28]  D. Wu Child Training in Chinese Culture , 1985 .

[29]  Jace Wolfe,et al.  Optimizing the benefit of sound processors coupled to personal FM systems. , 2008, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.

[30]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. , 2008 .

[31]  Khazriyati Salehuddin,et al.  Developmental milestone expectations, parenting styles and self construal in Malaysian and Australian caregivers , 2013 .

[32]  C. Yoshinaga-Itano,et al.  From Screening to Early Identification and Intervention: Discovering Predictors to Successful Outcomes for Children With Significant Hearing Loss. , 2003, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.

[33]  A. Bryk,et al.  Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. , 1991 .

[34]  Emily A Tobey,et al.  Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. , 2010, JAMA.

[35]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  How children use input to acquire a lexicon. , 2002, Child development.

[36]  Michelle M. Garrison,et al.  Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. , 2009, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[37]  John H L Hansen,et al.  Mapping the Early Language Environment Using All-Day Recordings and Automated Analysis. , 2017, American journal of speech-language pathology.

[38]  E. Hoff-Ginsberg Influences of mother and child on maternal talkativeness , 1994 .

[39]  Paige Stringer Issue of Sustainability in Global Humanitarian Programs , 2017 .

[40]  Betty Vohr,et al.  Assessing language and language environment of high‐risk infants and children: a new approach , 2013, Acta paediatrica.

[41]  Andrew M. Johnson,et al.  Language Outcomes in Children Who Are Deaf and Hard of Hearing: The Role of Language Ability Before Hearing Aid Intervention. , 2017, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[42]  A. Geers,et al.  Enduring advantages of early cochlear implantation for spoken language development. , 2013, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[43]  C. Flexer,et al.  Children with Hearing Loss: Developing Listening and Talking Birth to Six , 2007 .

[44]  Christine Yoshinaga-Itano,et al.  Using Language ENvironment Analysis to Improve Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing , 2012, Seminars in Speech and Language.

[45]  Soon-hyung Yi,et al.  The cultural context of talk about the past: Implications for the development of autobiographical memory ☆ ☆☆ , 1995 .

[46]  B. Olusanya,et al.  Benefits and challenges of newborn hearing screening for developing countries. , 2004, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.

[47]  Patrick Meehan,et al.  An Exploratory Study of “Quantitative Linguistic Feedback” , 2013 .

[48]  H. Keller,et al.  Socialization environments of Chinese and Euro-American middle-class babies: Parenting behaviors, verbal discourses and ethnotheories , 2007 .

[49]  J. Gilkerson,et al.  Assessing Children’s Home Language Environments Using Automatic Speech Recognition Technology , 2011 .

[50]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Talking to Children Matters , 2013, Psychological science.

[51]  Martha Crago Ethnography and language socialization: A cross‐cultural perspective , 1992 .

[52]  J. Singer,et al.  Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. , 2005, Child development.

[53]  M. Jit,et al.  Modeling the impact of rubella vaccination in Vietnam , 2016, Human vaccines & immunotherapeutics.

[54]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Differences in Early Gesture Explain SES Disparities in Child Vocabulary Size at School Entry , 2009, Science.

[55]  Lauri H. Nelson Deaf Education Services in Southern Regions of Vietnam: A Survey of Teacher Perceptions and Recommendations , 2015 .

[56]  Elinor Ochs Talking to children in Western Samoa , 1982, Language in Society.

[57]  Gene B. Halleck,et al.  Language socialization , 2008 .

[58]  Emma Brunskill,et al.  Global and regional hearing impairment prevalence: an analysis of 42 studies in 29 countries. , 2013, European journal of public health.

[59]  A. Varela,et al.  Cochlear Implants Versus Hearing Aids in a Middle-Income Country: Costs, Productivity, and Quality of Life. , 2017, Otology & neurotology : official publication of the American Otological Society, American Neurotology Society [and] European Academy of Otology and Neurotology.

[60]  D K Oller,et al.  Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[61]  M. Greenberg,et al.  Interactions of Deaf Mothers and Deaf Preschool Children: Comparisons with Three Other Groups of Deaf and Hearing Dyads , 1981, American annals of the deaf.

[62]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. , 2008, Developmental science.

[63]  Nguyen Duc Son,et al.  Parenting in Vietnam , 2014 .

[64]  M. P. Moeller,et al.  Language Outcomes in Young Children with Mild to Severe Hearing Loss , 2015, Ear and hearing.

[65]  M. Rowe,et al.  Predictors of Variation in Maternal Talk to Children: A Longitudinal Study of Low-Income Families , 2005 .

[66]  S. Warren,et al.  The role of maternal responsivity in the development of children with intellectual disabilities. , 2007, Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews.

[67]  C. Westby,et al.  Frameworks of Education: Perspectives of Southeast Asian Parents and Head Start Staff. , 2003, Language, speech, and hearing services in schools.

[68]  S. Warren,et al.  What Automated Vocal Analysis Reveals About the Vocal Production and Language Learning Environment of Young Children with Autism , 2010, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[69]  T. Ching,et al.  Language and speech outcomes of children with hearing loss and additional disabilities: identifying the variables that influence performance at five years of age , 2018, International journal of audiology.

[70]  Hugo Lagercrantz,et al.  Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: a two‐country study , 2013, Acta paediatrica.

[71]  R Leavitt,et al.  Speech degradation as measured by the Rapid Speech Transmission Index (RASTI). , 1991, Ear and hearing.

[72]  Stephanie M. Nixon,et al.  A Cross-cultural Mixed Methods Investigation of Language Socialization Practices. , 2019, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.

[73]  Rebecca J. Panagos Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children , 1998 .

[74]  B. McPherson,et al.  An international partnership analysis of a cohort of Vietnamese children with hearing impairment , 2016 .

[75]  J. Gilkerson,et al.  Teaching by Listening: The Importance of Adult-Child Conversations to Language Development , 2009, Pediatrics.

[76]  Heidi Waterfall,et al.  Sources of variability in children’s language growth , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.

[77]  J. Gilkerson,et al.  The impact of book reading in the early years on parent–child language interaction , 2017 .

[78]  M. Leichtman,et al.  Sharing memories and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their 3-year-olds , 2000, Memory.

[79]  C. Yoshinaga-Itano,et al.  The School to Home Link: Summer Preschool and Parents , 2012, Seminars in Speech and Language.

[80]  G. Quinn,et al.  Experimental Design and Data Analysis for Biologists , 2002 .

[81]  Jill Gilkerson,et al.  Vocal interaction between children with Down syndrome and their parents. , 2014, American journal of speech-language pathology.

[82]  Merav Shohet Everyday Sacrifice and Language Socialization in Vietnam: The Power of a Respect Particle , 2013 .

[83]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[84]  J. Johnston,et al.  Cultural differences in beliefs and practices concerning talk to children. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[85]  Jacqueline S. Thousand,et al.  Inclusion in Viet Nam: More Than a Decade of Implementation , 2003 .