Copying fideliy of functional and non-functional features in ni-Vanuatu children: A transmission chain study

Observational learning plays a key role in cultural transmission. Previous transmission chain experiments have shown that children are able to maintain information across multiple generations through observational learning. It still remains unclear how the transmission of functional vs. non-functional information and the effect of being observed unfold across age in different communities. Here, we examine children’s copying fidelity in observational learning of 5- to 13-year-olds from five different communities in Vanuatu, both individually (n = 263, 144 boys) and throughout a transmission chain of five to six children (n = 324, 178 boys). We additionally varied the functionality of the feature being copied (shape vs. color) and the copying context (observed vs. unobserved). Further, we also study developmental and cultural variation in the interaction of features and conditions. We find that children transmit the functional feature shape more faithfully than the non-functional feature color, both in the dyadic transitions as well as the transmission chains with an increasing tendency to do so as they get older. The age patterns show greater variation between communities for color than for shape. Overall, we find that being observed shows no uniform effects but influences transmission differently across communities. Our study shows that children are prone to passing on a functional feature across multiple generations of peers. Children copy non-functional features as well, but with lower fidelity. In sum, our results show children’s high propensity and developing abilities for observational learning, ultimately allowing for effective cultural transmission.

[1]  R. Mcelreath,et al.  Expanding the understanding of majority-bias in children’s social learning , 2022, Scientific reports.

[2]  Julia M. Rohrer,et al.  A Causal Framework for Cross-Cultural Generalizability , 2021, Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science.

[3]  R. Mcelreath,et al.  Conformity decreases throughout middle childhood among ni-Vanuatu children: An intracultural comparison. , 2021, Developmental psychology.

[4]  Robyn M. Holmes,et al.  Cultural Learning , 2020, Cultural Psychology.

[5]  D. Haun,et al.  Being observed increases overimitation in three diverse cultures. , 2019, Developmental psychology.

[6]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Are you watching me? The role of audience and object novelty in overimitation. , 2019, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  A. Whiten,et al.  ‘Over-imitation’: A review and appraisal of a decade of research , 2019, Developmental Review.

[8]  C. Tennie,et al.  Young children fail to generate an additive ratchet effect in an open-ended construction task , 2018, PloS one.

[9]  Tanya Broesch,et al.  Variation is the universal: making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology , 2018, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[10]  D. Reich,et al.  Population Turnover in Remote Oceania Shortly after Initial Settlement , 2018, Current Biology.

[11]  R. Gray,et al.  Language continuity despite population replacement in Remote Oceania , 2018, Nature Ecology & Evolution.

[12]  Amanda J. Lucas,et al.  Innovation and social transmission in experimental micro-societies: exploring the scope of cumulative culture in young children , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  K. Laland,et al.  Sex differences in confidence influence patterns of conformity , 2017, British journal of psychology.

[14]  Joscha Kärtner,et al.  The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. , 2017, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[15]  C Tennie,et al.  Young children copy cumulative technological design in the absence of action information , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[16]  C. Legare,et al.  A cross-cultural comparison of children's imitative flexibility. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[17]  D. Geary,et al.  Evolutionary developmental psychology. , 2015, Child development.

[18]  K. Laland,et al.  The development of adaptive conformity in young children: effects of uncertainty and consensus. , 2015, Developmental science.

[19]  Aiko Hanayama,et al.  Boys, Be Independent! Conformity Development of Japanese Children in the Asch Experiment without Using Confederates , 2014 .

[20]  M. Carpenter,et al.  Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[21]  A. François The dynamics of linguistic diversity: egalitarian multilingualism and power imbalance among northern Vanuatu languages , 2012 .

[22]  Cara DiYanni,et al.  Looking good versus doing good: which factors take precedence when children learn about new tools? , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  D. Bjorklund,et al.  Evolutionary developmental psychology. , 2010, Psicothema.

[24]  Kathleen H Corriveau,et al.  Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[25]  Christine A Caldwell,et al.  Social Learning Mechanisms and Cumulative Cultural Evolution , 2009, Psychological science.

[26]  A. Whiten,et al.  Cultural Transmission of Tool Use in Young Children: A Diffusion Chain Study , 2008 .

[27]  C. Caldwell,et al.  Experimental models for testing hypotheses about cumulative cultural evolution , 2008 .

[28]  Andrew G. Young,et al.  The hidden structure of overimitation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  Doing the right thing: infants' selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. , 2007, Child development.

[30]  R. Mace,et al.  A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[31]  C. Heyes,et al.  Infants' behavioral reenactment of "failed attempts": exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus enhancement, and understanding of intentions. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[32]  J. Lynch South Efate Phonological History , 2000 .

[33]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[34]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of Gestures in Children is Goal-directed , 2000 .

[35]  O. Bagasra,et al.  Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 1914, Science.

[36]  P. Richerson,et al.  The Evolution of Ethnic Markers , 1987 .

[37]  J. R. Landis,et al.  The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. , 1977, Biometrics.

[38]  N. Hamm A partial test of a social learning theory of children's conformity , 1970 .

[39]  Carla H. Lagorio,et al.  Psychology , 1929, Nature.

[40]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .