Individual differences in prefrontal cortical activation on the Tower of London planning task: implication for effortful processing

Solving challenging (‘effortful’) problems is known to involve the dorsal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in normal volunteers, although there is considerable individual variation. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging study, we show that healthy subjects with different levels of performance in the Tower of London planning task exhibit different patterns of brain activation. All subjects exhibited significant bilateral activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior and posterior cingulate areas and the parietal cortex. However, ‘standard performers’ (performance < 70% correct) and ‘superior performers’ (performance >70% correct) differed in the patterns of activation exhibited. Superior performers showed a significantly more spatially extended activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex than did standard performers, whereas the latter group tended to show increased activation of the anterior cingulate region.

[1]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[2]  C. Frith,et al.  Prefrontal dysfunction in depressed patients performing a complex planning task: a study using positron emission tomography , 1997, Psychological Medicine.

[3]  B. Postle,et al.  Maintenance versus Manipulation of Information Held in Working Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[5]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Influence of Working-Memory Demand and Subject Performance on Prefrontal Cortical Activity , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  L. Hasher,et al.  Automatic and effortful processes in memory. , 1979 .

[7]  H. Barbas Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and emotion in primate prefrontal cortices , 2000, Brain Research Bulletin.

[8]  B. Bussel,et al.  Longitudinal Study of Motor Recovery After Stroke: Recruitment and Focusing of Brain Activation , 2002, Stroke.

[9]  V. Schmithorst,et al.  Changes in neuronal activation with increasing attention demand in healthy volunteers: An fMRI study , 2001, Synapse.

[10]  M D'Esposito,et al.  The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: effects of memory load and individual differences. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  F Barkhof,et al.  Visualizing brain activation during planning: the tower of London test adapted for functional MR imaging. , 2000, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[12]  P. Goldman-Rakic Architecture of the Prefrontal Cortex and the Central Executive , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  From the Selectedworks of Marcel Adam Just Working Memory and Executive Function: Evidence from Neuroimaging Classic Issues Neuroimaging Results Working Memory and Executive Function: Evidence from Neuroimaging Reconstruals Suggested by the Neuroimaging Data Collaboration and Redundancy , 2022 .

[14]  M. Rugg,et al.  When more means less neural activity related to unsuccessful memory encoding , 2001, Current Biology.

[15]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Neurobiology of attention and automaticity , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  The Strategic Control of Gaze Direction in the Tower of London Task , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  G. Bruyn Atlas of the Cerebral Sulci, M. Ono, S. Kubik, Chad D. Abernathey (Eds.). Georg Thieme Verlag, Stuttgart, New York (1990), 232, DM 298 , 1990 .

[19]  B. Postle,et al.  Evaluating models of the topographical organization of working memory function in frontal cortex with event-related fMRI , 2000, Psychobiology.

[20]  S Dehaene,et al.  A hierarchical neuronal network for planning behavior. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  E. Crelin Atlas of Human Anatomy , 1965, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[23]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[25]  D. Manoach,et al.  Prefrontal cortex fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load , 1997, Neuroreport.

[26]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  A. Owen Tuning in to the temporal dynamics of brain activation using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Charles L Truwit,et al.  High Resolution Atlas of Cranial Neuroanatomy , 1994 .

[30]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Abnormal basal ganglia outflow in Parkinson's disease identified with PET. Implications for higher cortical functions. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Imaging the mental components of a planning task , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  E. Miller,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex Complex Neural Properties for Complex Behavior , 1999, Neuron.

[33]  R. Krikorian,et al.  Tower of London procedure: a standard method and developmental data. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[34]  S Dehaene,et al.  A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[36]  R. Coppola,et al.  Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  S. Petersen,et al.  Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Role of the human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor, manual, and speech responses: a positron emission tomography study. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  小野 道夫,et al.  Atlas of the Cerebral Sulci , 1990 .

[40]  R. Morris,et al.  Neural correlates of planning ability: Frontal lobe activation during the tower of London test , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and the Monitoring of Response Conflict: Evidence from an fMRI Study of Overt Verb Generation , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  The role of the lateral frontal cortex in mnemonic processing: the contribution of functional neuroimaging , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of high-level cognition in Parkinson's disease: the role of the prefrontal cortex revealed by PET. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal , 2001, Nature.

[45]  S. Petersen,et al.  The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Planning Processes and Age in the Five-disc Tower of London Task , 1999 .

[47]  S. Ogawa,et al.  Insights into new techniques for high resolution functional MRI , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[48]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  G. Ward,et al.  Planning and Problem solving Using the Five disc Tower of London Task , 1997 .

[50]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex—An Update Time Is of the Essence , 2001, Neuron.

[51]  A. Scheibel,et al.  The Human Brain Coloring Book , 1985 .

[52]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[53]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[54]  J. Tanji,et al.  Behavioral planning in the prefrontal cortex , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.