Effect of Instructions on Spatial Visualisation Ability in Civil Engineering Students

Spatial visualisation ability is a subset of spatial ability - one of the factors of human intelligence structure. Spatial visualisation ability has been defined as, " the ability to mentally manipulate, rotate, twist, or invert pictorially presented stimulus objects." (McGee, 1979, p. 893). This multi-faceted ability helps engineers including civil engineers to conceptualise links between reality and the abstract model of that reality. For example, in the analysis of a loaded beam structure, the deflected shape of the beam is the reality and the mathematical equation chosen (often unseen in a computer design package) to represent this deflected shape is the abstract model of that reality. A civil engineer needs first to have an intuitive understanding of the interactive relationships among structural components before he could begin to predict the deflected shape, which leads to the identification of the abstract model. This intuitive understanding is also essential to civil engineers in another way. It helps them in visualising, predicting, designing and checking for the worst possible combination of loads on a given structure. In the design of a reinforced concrete design for example, the ability to visualise aids the engineer in the prediction of critical loading conditions, which is necessary in reaching an adequate reinforcement design. Spatial visualisation ability has also been found to be essential to a student's success in some engineering related subjects such as calculus (Winkle, 1997), mathematics, (Battista,Wheatley and Talsma, 1989), engineering drawing (Sorby and Baartmans, 1996a) and computer-aided design (Sorby, 1999). Early indication for a positive relationship between this ability and structural design has also been found. Alias (2000) carried out a learning task analysis on the design of a reinforced concrete column - an area that demands a wide variety of structural design skills - and discovered that spatial skills are pre-requisite to the learning of column design suggesting that spatial visualisation ability may be essential to the success of structural design. Overall, spatial visualisation ability may be critical to learning and problem solving in civil engineering especially to the learning of subject matter that demands spatial strategies such as structural design, engineering mathematics and soil mechanics. There has also been some suggestions that spatial ability is relevant to problem solving that is not dependent on spatial strategies proposed by Roberts, Gilmore and Wood (1997). Roberts, et al. proposed that in cases where non-spatial strategies are required, spatial ability influences the degree to which a problem solver is able to develop and evaluate these strategies. In summary, spatial visualisation ability appears to be necessary to problem solving in engineering related areas that requires spatial as well as non-spatial strategies. Therefore, a better understanding of this ability should be potentially beneficial to the engineering education and profession.

[1]  John A. Deno The Relationship of Previous Experiences to Spatial Visualization Ability. , 1995 .

[2]  M. G. McGee Human spatial abilities: psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  T. Lord Enhancing the visuo‐spatial aptitude of students , 1985 .

[4]  Melvin J. Pedras,et al.  A Comparison of Two Methods of Teaching Visualization Skills to College Students. , 1993 .

[5]  Maizam Alias,et al.  Spatial visualisation ability and problem solving in civil engineering , 2000 .

[6]  Sheryl A. Sorby,et al.  Making Connections: Spatial Skills and Engineering Drawings. , 1996 .

[7]  C. R. Killian,et al.  Cognitive development of college freshmen , 1979 .

[8]  Anthony S. Bryk,et al.  Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods , 1992 .

[9]  B. Bowonder,et al.  Emerging trends in information technology: Implications for developing countries , 1993 .

[10]  G. M. Seddon,et al.  The Understanding of Pictorial Depth Cues, and the Ability to Visualise the Rotation of Three‐dimensional Structures in Diagrams , 1986 .

[11]  P. Sutherland,et al.  The application of Piagetian and Neo-Piagetian ideas to further and higher education , 1999 .

[12]  K. E. Shubbar Learning the Visualisation of Rotations in Diagrams of Three Dimensional Structures. , 1990 .

[13]  Katherine Schultz,et al.  The contribution of solution strategy to spatial performance , 1991 .

[14]  David M. Brohn Understanding Structural Analysis , 1984 .

[15]  Gerald Zaltman,et al.  Innovation and Organizations. , 1974 .

[16]  I Gusti,et al.  Adoption and implementation of information technology in Bali's local government: A comparison between single level path analyses using PLSPATH 3.01 and AMOS 4 and Multilevel Path Analyses using MPLUS 2.01 , 2001 .

[17]  M. J. Allen,et al.  Spatial Problem-Solving Strategies as Functions of Sex , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[18]  B. Winkel In plane view: an exercise in visualization , 1997 .

[19]  G. Seddon,et al.  The Effects of Colour in Teaching the Visualisation of Rotations in Diagrams of Three‐dimensional Structures , 1985 .

[20]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[21]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[22]  D. J. Gilmore,et al.  Individual differences and strategy selection in reasoning , 1997 .

[23]  G. M. Seddon,et al.  Learning the Visualisation of Three-Dimensional Spatial Relationships in Diagrams at Different Ages in Bahrain. , 1985 .

[24]  Marian Louise Tillotson,et al.  The effect of instruction in spatial visualization on spatial abilities and mathematical problem solving , 1984 .

[25]  I Gusti,et al.  The change of structural, perception and attitudinal dimensions in information technology implementation processes , 2000 .

[26]  L. G. Tornatzky,et al.  Innovation characteristics and innovation adoption-implementation: A meta-analysis of findings , 1982, IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management.

[27]  Susan Lee Zavotka,et al.  Three-dimensional computer animated graphics: A tool for spatial skill instruction , 1987 .

[28]  G. Seddon,et al.  The Visualization of Rotation in Diagrams of Three-Dimensional Structures , 1984 .

[29]  M. Peters,et al.  Performance of male and female children, adolescents and adults on spatial tasks that involve everyday objects and settings. , 1993, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[30]  C. Reesink,et al.  Secondary students' performance on Piagetian projective and Euclidean spatial tasks. , 1985, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[31]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[32]  Sheryl A. Sorby,et al.  Improving The 3 D Spatial Visualization Skills Of Women Engineering Students , 1996 .

[33]  John L. Phillips,et al.  The Origins of Intellect: Piaget's Theory , 1975 .

[34]  Ming-Te Lu,et al.  Information systems development in developing countries: An evaluation and recommendations , 1990 .

[35]  John Izard,et al.  Developing Spatial Skills with Three-Dimensional Puzzles. , 1990 .

[36]  C J Hamilton,et al.  Beyond sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: the impact of gender trait possession. , 1995, British journal of psychology.

[37]  Thomas Black Doing Quantitative Research in the Social Sciences: An Integrated Approach to Research Design, Measurement and Statistics , 1999 .

[38]  Richard T. Houang,et al.  The Effect of Instruction on Spatial Visualization Skills of Middle School Boys and Girls , 1988 .

[39]  Michael T. Battista,et al.  Spatial Visualization, Formal Reasoning, and Geometric Problem-Solving Strategies of Preservice Elementary Teachers. , 1989 .

[40]  Sheryl A. Sorby,et al.  A Course for the Development of 3-D Spatial Visualization Skills. , 1996 .