What determines the specificity of conflict adaptation? A review, critical analysis, and proposed synthesis

Over the past decade, many cognitive control researchers have studied to what extent adaptations to conflict are domain-general or rather specific, mostly by testing whether or not the congruency sequence effect (CSE) transfers across different conditions (e.g., conflict type, task sets, contexts, et cetera). The CSE refers to the observation that congruency effects in conflict tasks tend to be reduced following incongruent relative to following congruent trials, and is considered a prime measure of cognitive control. By investigating the transfer of this CSE across different conflict types, tasks, or contexts, researchers made several inferences about the scope of cognitive control. This method gained popularity during the last few years, spawning an interesting, yet seemingly inconsistent set of results. Consequently, these observations gave rise to a number of equally divergent theories about the determinants and scope of conflict adaptation. In this review, we offer a systematic overview of these past studies, as well as an evaluation of the theories that have been put forward to account for the results. Finally, we propose an integration of these various theoretical views in a unifying framework that centers on the role of context (dis)similarity. This framework allows us to generate new predictions about the relation between task or context similarity and the scope of cognitive control. Specifically, while most theories imply that increasing contextual differences will result in reduced transfer of the CSE, we propose that context similarity and across-context control follow a U-shaped function instead.

[1]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[2]  A. Freitas,et al.  Contextual Adjustments in Cognitive Control Across Tasks , 2007, Psychological science.

[3]  L. Jiménez,et al.  Even with time, conflict adaptation is not made of expectancies , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[4]  E. Maylor,et al.  Multiple systems for cognitive control: evidence from a hybrid prime-Simon task. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Generalized signaling for control: evidence from postconflict and posterror performance adjustments. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. , 2008, Psychological review.

[7]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  A formal model of capacity limits in working memory , 2006 .

[9]  Simon Farrell,et al.  Modeling working memory: An interference model of complex span , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Cognitive control acts locally , 2008, Cognition.

[11]  H. Haider,et al.  That’s what task sets are for: shielding against irrelevant information , 2008, Psychological research.

[12]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  T. Egner,et al.  Determinants of congruency sequence effects without learning and memory confounds. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[15]  Yang Seok Cho,et al.  Congruency sequence effect in cross-task context: evidence for dimension-specific modulation. , 2013, Acta Psychologica.

[16]  W. Notebaert,et al.  The heterogeneous world of congruency sequence effects: an update , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[17]  Ran R. Hassin,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General The Control-Freak Mind : Stereotypical Biases Are Eliminated Following Conflict-Activated Cognitive Control , 2013 .

[18]  E. Hazeltine,et al.  Domain-specific conflict adaptation without feature repetitions , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  Petroc Sumner,et al.  Unconscious inhibition separates two forms of cognitive control , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Sabine Schwager,et al.  Across-task conflict regulation: a replication failure. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Yang Seok Cho,et al.  Congruency sequence effect without feature integration and contingency learning. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[23]  E. Hazeltine,et al.  Conflict adaptation depends on task structure. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  H. Haider,et al.  How task representations guide attention: further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  D. Fernandez-Duque,et al.  Cognitive control: dynamic, sustained, and voluntary influences. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Hillary D. Schwarb,et al.  The boundaries of sequential modulations: evidence for set-level control. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  W. Kunde,et al.  A Simon effect for stimulus-response duration , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[28]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[29]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  Now you see it, now you don't: controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions eliminates the Gratton effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[30]  Michiel M. A. Spapé,et al.  He said, she said: Episodic retrieval induces conflict adaptation in an auditory Stroop task , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  Jeounghoon Kim,et al.  Conflict adjustment through domain-specific multiple cognitive control mechanisms , 2012, Brain Research.

[32]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Sequential modulations of interference evoked by processing task-irrelevant stimulus features. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[34]  H. Haider,et al.  The role of task rules and stimulus–response mappings in the task switching paradigm , 2007, Psychological research.

[35]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Adaptation by binding: a learning account of cognitive control , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  T. Egner Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Conflict adaptation: It is not what you expect , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  Congruency Sequence Effects without Feature Integration or Contingency Learning Confounds , 2014, PloS one.

[39]  Baptist Liefooghe,et al.  Automatic motor activation by mere instruction , 2014, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[40]  James R. Schmidt Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Conflict adaptation by means of associative learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[43]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Instruction-based task-rule congruency effects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  W. Kunde,et al.  Sequential modulations of correspondence effects across spatial dimensions and tasks , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[45]  W. Kunde,et al.  Adaptation to (non)valent task disturbance , 2012, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Reward determines the context-sensitivity of cognitive control. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  W. Notebaert,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Generalizing Attentional Control across Dimensions and Tasks: Evidence from Transfer of Proportion- Congruent Effects View Related Articles View Crossmark Data Citing Articles: 1 View Citing Articles , 2022 .

[48]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  A. Freitas,et al.  Generality and specificity in cognitive control: conflict adaptation within and across selective-attention tasks but not across selective-attention and Simon tasks , 2015, Psychological research.

[50]  Irene P. Kan,et al.  To adapt or not to adapt: The question of domain-general cognitive control , 2013, Cognition.

[51]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Trial-to-trial modulations of the Simon effect in conditions of attentional limitations: Evidence from dual tasks. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  Dorret I. Boomsma,et al.  Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: Conflict adaptation or associative priming? , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[53]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Tasks, Task Sets, and the Mapping Between Them , 2014 .

[54]  Raymond Y. Cho,et al.  Context Specificity of Post-Error and Post-Conflict Cognitive Control Adjustments , 2014, PloS one.

[55]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain , 2007, NeuroImage.

[56]  B. Hommel,et al.  A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.

[57]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[58]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Evidence for task-specific resolution of response conflict , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[59]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Sustained vs. transient cognitive control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation , 2010, Cognition.