A shared code for Braille and Arabic digits revealed by cross-modal priming in sighted Braille readers.

Quantities can be represented by different formats (e.g. symbolic or non-symbolic) and conveyed via different modalities (e.g. tactile or visual). Despite different priming curves: V-shape and step-shape for place and summation coded representation, respectively, the occurrence of priming effect supports the notion of different format overlap on the same mental number line. However, little is known about tactile-visual overlap of symbolic numerosities i.e. Braille numbers to Arabic digits on the magnitude number representation. Here, in a priming experiment, we tested a unique group of sighted Braille readers to investigate whether tactile Braille digits would activate a place-coding type of mental number representation (V-shape), analogous to other symbolic formats. The primes were either tactile Braille digits presented on a Braille display or number words presented on a computer screen. The targets were visually presented Arabic digits, and subjects performed a naming task. Our results reveal a V-shape priming function for both prime formats: tactile Braille and written words representing numbers, with strongest priming for primes of identical value (e.g. "four" and "4"), and a symmetrical decrease of priming strength for neighboring numbers, which indicates that the observed priming is due to identity priming. We thus argue that the magnitude information is processed according to a shared phonological code, independent of the input modality.

[1]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Tactile enumeration of small quantities using one hand. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[2]  S. Millar Reading by touch , 1997 .

[3]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[4]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990 .

[5]  T. Verguts,et al.  Dissecting the symbolic distance effect: Comparison and priming effects in numerical and nonnumerical orders , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  Marcin Szwed,et al.  Braille in the Sighted: Teaching Tactile Reading to Sighted Adults , 2016, PloS one.

[7]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  F. Restle Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .

[9]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[11]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  A Feeling for Numbers: Shared Metric for Symbolic and Tactile Numerosities , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[12]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Tactile spatial resolution in blind Braille readers , 2000, Neurology.

[13]  A. Knops,et al.  No Conclusive Evidence for Numerical Priming Under Interocular Suppression , 2014, Psychological science.

[14]  E. Spelke,et al.  Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.

[15]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  P. Sterzer,et al.  Exploring the boundary conditions of unconscious numerical priming effects with continuous flash suppression , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[18]  André Knops,et al.  Probing the Neural Correlates of Number Processing , 2017, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[19]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  R. Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Numerical representation in the parietal lobes: abstract or not abstract? , 2009, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[21]  Wim Fias,et al.  Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  Amir Amedi,et al.  Massive cortical reorganization in sighted Braille readers , 2016, eLife.

[23]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  J. L. Myers,et al.  Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Semantic priming in number naming , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[27]  Michaël A. Stevens,et al.  A model of exact small-number representation , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional and Structural Alterations of the Intraparietal Sulcus in a Developmental Dyscalculia of Genetic Origin , 2003, Neuron.

[29]  M. H. Fischer,et al.  Stimulating numbers: signatures of finger counting in numerosity processing , 2020, Psychological research.

[30]  Wim Fias,et al.  Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities , 2007, Cognition.

[31]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Primed numbers : Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming , 1999 .

[32]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line , 1999, Cognition.