A longitudinal study of gender-related cognition and behaviour.

Gender schema theory proposes that children's acquisition of gender labels and gender stereotypes informs gender-congruent behaviour. Most previous studies have been cross-sectional and do not address the temporal relationship between knowledge and behaviour. We report the results of a longitudinal study of gender knowledge and sex-typed behaviour across three domains in children tested at 24 and 36 months (N = 56). Although both knowledge and sex-typed behaviour increased significantly between 2 and 3 years, there was no systematic pattern of cross-lagged correlations between the two, although some concurrent relationships were present at 24 months. Future longitudinal work might profitably focus on younger children using reliable preverbal measures of gender knowledge and employing a shorter lag between measurement times.

[1]  J. Strayer,et al.  An ethological analysis of social agonism and dominance relations among preschool children. , 1976 .

[2]  B. Fagot Beyond the reinforcement principle: Another step toward understanding sex role development. , 1985 .

[3]  Peter K. Smith A longitudinal study of social participation in preschool children: Solitary and parallel play reexamined. , 1978 .

[4]  Zolinda Stoneman,et al.  Naturalistic observations of children's activities and roles while playing with their siblings and friends. , 1984 .

[5]  J. Benenson,et al.  Gender Differences in Emotional Closeness Between Preschool Children and Their Mothers , 1998 .

[6]  B. Fagot Changes in thinking about early sex role development , 1985 .

[7]  C. Martin,et al.  New Directions for Investigating Children′s Gender Knowledge , 1993 .

[8]  Adam J. White,et al.  Does Early Sex Typing Result from Children's Attempts to Match Their Behavior to Sex Role Stereotypes?. , 1984 .

[9]  D. Coker The relationships among gender concepts and cognitive maturity in preschool children , 1984 .

[10]  B. Thorne,et al.  SEXUALITY AND GENDER IN CHILDREN'S DAILY WORLDS* , 1986 .

[11]  B. Fagot,et al.  Categorical habituation to male and female faces: Gender schematic processing in infancy** , 1993 .

[12]  C. Edelbrock,et al.  Acquisition of Sex-Typed Preferences in Preschool-Aged Children. , 1978 .

[13]  J. Benenson,et al.  Propulsion : A behavioural expression of masculinity , 1997 .

[14]  D. B. Carter,et al.  Cognitive aspects of early sex-role development: The influence of gender schemas on preschoolers' memories and preferences for sex-typed toys and activities. , 1988 .

[15]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Sex differences in human neonatal social perception , 2000 .

[16]  C. Martin,et al.  The development of gender stereotype components. , 1990, Child development.

[17]  V. Morgan,et al.  Nursery and Infant School Play Patterns: sex‐related differences , 1987 .

[18]  L. S. Liben,et al.  Cognitive mechanisms in children's gender stereotyping: theoretical and educational implications of a cognitive-based intervention. , 1992, Child development.

[19]  C. Howes Peer interaction of young children , 1989 .

[20]  J. Mandler,et al.  Concept formation in infancy , 1993 .

[21]  M. Hines,et al.  Gender labels and play styles: their relative contribution to children's selection of playmates. , 1994, Child development.

[22]  B. Fagot Teacher and peer reactions to boys' and girls' play styles , 1984 .

[23]  K. Flannery,et al.  Are individual differences in fantasy play related to peer acceptance levels? , 1993, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[24]  B. Fagot,et al.  Is there coherence among the cognitive components of gender acquisition? , 1991 .

[25]  B. Fagot,et al.  The young child's gender schema: environmental input, internal organization. , 1989, Child development.

[26]  G. Levy Gender-Typed and Non-Gender-Typed Category Awareness in Toddlers , 1999 .

[27]  A. Pellegrini,et al.  Classroom contextual effects on children's play. , 1989 .

[28]  S. Berenbaum,et al.  Early hormonal influences on childhood sex-typed activity and playmate preferences: implications for the development of sexual orientation , 1995 .

[29]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  Age‐related Asynchrony of Knowledge and Action , 1991 .

[30]  Yvonne M. Caldera,et al.  Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, masculine, and neutral toys. , 1989, Child development.

[31]  J. Lever,et al.  Sex Differences in the Games Children Play , 1976 .

[32]  B. Fagot,et al.  Gender labeling and the adoption of sex-typed behaviors. , 1986 .

[33]  Cynthia L. Miller Qualitative differences among gender-stereotyped toys: Implications for cognitive and social development in girls and boys , 1987 .

[34]  J. L. Roopnarine Mothers' and fathers' behaviors toward the toy play of their infant sons and daughters , 1986 .

[35]  B. Fagot,et al.  Gender-role development in young children : From discrimination to labeling , 1993 .

[36]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  GPOWER: A general power analysis program , 1996 .

[37]  L. S. Liben,et al.  The role of attitudes and interventions in gender-schematic processing. , 1990, Child development.

[38]  J. DiPietro,et al.  Rough and tumble play: A function of gender. , 1981 .

[39]  E. Maccoby,et al.  Gender as a social category. , 1988 .

[40]  B. J. Myers,et al.  The development of sex role stereotypes in the third year: relationships to gender labeling, gender identity, sex-typed toy preference, and family characteristics. , 1984, Child development.

[41]  N. Eisenberg-Berg,et al.  Correlates of Preschool Girls' Feminine and Masculine Toy Preferences. , 1979 .

[42]  Charles F. Halverson,et al.  A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. , 1981 .

[43]  R. Hinde,et al.  Gender‐typing1 in young children: Preferences, behaviour and cultural differences , 1993 .