Cooperative and Opposing Effects of Strategic and Involuntary Attention
暂无分享,去创建一个
Leonardo Chelazzi | Massimo Turatto | Veronica Mazza | Monica Dallabona | L. Chelazzi | M. Turatto | Monica Dallabona | V. Mazza
[1] S J Luck,et al. Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Tracking the Location of Visuospatial Attention in a Contingent Capture Paradigm , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[3] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Attentional selection and identification of visual objects are reflected by distinct electrophysiological responses , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] Martin Eimer,et al. Involuntary Attentional Capture is Determined by Task Set: Evidence from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[5] Agnieszka Wykowska,et al. On the Temporal Relation of Top–Down and Bottom–Up Mechanisms during Guidance of Attention , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] E. Vogel,et al. Interactions between attention and working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.
[7] Maro G. Machizawa,et al. Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.
[8] Paul E. Downing,et al. Competition in visual working memory for control of search , 2003 .
[9] Jeff Miller,et al. Using the jackknife-based scoring method for measuring LRP onset effects in factorial designs. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[10] G. Humphreys,et al. Automatic guidance of attention from working memory , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[11] P. Downing,et al. Interactions Between Visual Working Memory and Selective Attention , 2000, Psychological science.
[12] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[13] Jeff Miller,et al. Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[14] Christian N L Olivers,et al. Interactions between visual working memory and visual attention. , 2008, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.
[15] Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al. Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search , 1999, Nature.
[16] C. Bundesen. A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.
[17] Clayton Hickey,et al. Priming resolves perceptual ambiguity in visual search: Evidence from behaviour and electrophysiology , 2010, Vision Research.
[18] G. Humphreys,et al. Dissociating the neural mechanisms of memory-based guidance of visual selection , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[19] G. Humphreys,et al. Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Martin Eimer,et al. Priming of pop-out modulates attentional target selection in visual search: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence , 2010, Vision Research.
[21] J. Theeuwes,et al. Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: visual working memory content affects visual attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] Jason T. Arita,et al. A cuing study of the N2pc component: An index of attentional deployment to objects rather than spatial locations , 2009, Brain Research.
[23] G. Woodman,et al. Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc , 2009, Cortex.
[25] H Pashler,et al. Do images involuntarily trigger search? A test of Pillsbury’s hypothesis , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[26] David Soto,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for attentional guidance by the contents of working memory , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[27] Eric Ruthruff,et al. Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: converging evidence from event-related potentials. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[29] R Verleger,et al. Lateralized human cortical activity for shifting visuospatial attention and initiating saccades. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[30] T. Egner,et al. Working memory as internal attention: Toward an integrative account of internal and external selection processes , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[31] A. Caramazza,et al. An electrophysiological assessment of distractor suppression in visual search tasks. , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[32] M. Eimer. The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[33] J. Theeuwes,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[34] S. Luck,et al. Neural sources of focused attention in visual search. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[35] P. Roelfsema,et al. The effect of items in working memory on the deployment of attention and the eyes during visual search. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] S. Luck,et al. Bridging the Gap between Monkey Neurophysiology and Human Perception: An Ambiguity Resolution Theory of Visual Selective Attention , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.
[37] Nicolas Robitaille,et al. On the control of visual spatial attention: evidence from human electrophysiology , 2006, Psychological research.
[38] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence of Semantic Interference in Visual Search , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] L. Chelazzi,et al. Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.