Representational Issues in the Debate on the Standard Model of the Mind

In this paper we discuss some of the issues concerning the Memory and Content aspects in the recent debate on the identification of a Standard Model of the Mind (Laird, Lebiere, and Rosenbloom in press). In particular, we focus on the representational models concerning the Declarative Memories of current Cognitive Architectures (CAs). In doing so we outline some of the main problems affecting the current CAs and suggest that the Conceptual Spaces, a representational framework developed by Gardenfors, is worth-considering to address such problems. Finally, we briefly analyze the alternative representational assumptions employed in the three CAs constituting the current baseline for the Standard Model (i.e. SOAR, ACT-R and Sigma). In doing so, we point out the respective differences and discuss their implications in the light of the analyzed problems.

[1]  Antonio Lieto,et al.  The knowledge level in cognitive architectures: Current limitations and possible developments , 2018, Cognitive Systems Research.

[2]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts , 1981, Cognition.

[3]  Christian Balkenius,et al.  Spaces in the Brain: From Neurons to Meanings , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[4]  G. G. Stokes "J." , 1890, The New Yale Book of Quotations.

[5]  Daniele P. Radicioni,et al.  A Common-Sense Conceptual Categorization System Integrating Heterogeneous Proxytypes and the Dual Process of Reasoning , 2015, IJCAI.

[6]  N. Hari Narayanan,et al.  Diagrammatic Reasoning: Cognitive and Computational Perspectives , 1995 .

[7]  Richard Reviewer-Granger Unified Theories of Cognition , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Christian Lebiere,et al.  How Limited Systematicity Emerges: A Computational Cognitive Neuroscience Approach (Author's Manuscript) , 2014 .

[9]  John E. Laird,et al.  The Soar Cognitive Architecture , 2012 .

[10]  Daniele P. Radicioni,et al.  Dual PECCS: a cognitive system for conceptual representation and categorization , 2017, J. Exp. Theor. Artif. Intell..

[11]  Antonio Lieto,et al.  Representing Concepts in Formal Ontologies: Compositionality vs. Typicality Effects" , 2013 .

[12]  Shimon Edelman,et al.  Representation, similarity, and the chorus of prototypes , 1993, Minds and Machines.

[13]  Stuart M. Rodgers,et al.  Integrating ACT-R and Cyc in a large-scale model of language comprehension for use in intelligent agents , 2004 .

[14]  John E. Laird,et al.  A Standard Model of the Mind: Toward a Common Computational Framework across Artificial Intelligence, Cognitive Science, Neuroscience, and Robotics , 2017, AI Mag..

[15]  Jonathan Lawry,et al.  Hierarchical conceptual spaces for concept combination , 2016, Artif. Intell..

[16]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Conceptual spaces - the geometry of thought , 2000 .

[17]  Salvatore Gaglio,et al.  Anchoring symbols to conceptual spaces: the case of dynamic scenarios , 2003, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[18]  Silvia Coradeschi,et al.  Perceptual anchoring via conceptual spaces , 2004, AAAI 2004.

[19]  John R Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of the mind. , 2004, Psychological review.

[20]  P ? ? ? ? ? ? ? % ? ? ? ? , 1991 .

[21]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  Applications of conceptual spaces : the case for geometric knowledge representation , 2015 .

[22]  Antonio Lieto,et al.  A Computational Framework for Concept Representation in Cognitive Systems and Architectures: Concepts as Heterogeneous Proxytypes , 2014, BICA.

[23]  Dario D. Salvucci Endowing a Cognitive Architecture with World Knowledge , 2014, CogSci.

[24]  Alessandro Oltramari,et al.  Pursuing Artificial General Intelligence by Leveraging the Knowledge Capabilities of ACT-R , 2012, AGI.

[25]  John E. Laird,et al.  Towards efficiently supporting large symbolic declarative memories , 2010 .

[26]  Aaron Sloman,et al.  How Can We Reduce the Gulf between Artificial and Natural Intelligence? , 2014, AIC.

[27]  Antonio Chella,et al.  Conceptual Spaces for Cognitive Architectures: A Lingua Franca for Different Levels of Representation , 2017, BICA 2017.

[28]  Steven Schockaert,et al.  Inducing semantic relations from conceptual spaces: A data-driven approach to plausible reasoning , 2015, Artif. Intell..

[29]  Henry Markram,et al.  Cliques of Neurons Bound into Cavities Provide a Missing Link between Structure and Function , 2016, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[30]  Steven Jones,et al.  Efficient Computation of Spreading Activation Using Lazy Evaluation , 2016 .

[31]  Enrico Mensa,et al.  A Resource-Driven Approach for Anchoring Linguistic Resources to Conceptual Spaces , 2016, AI*IA.

[32]  J. Fodor,et al.  Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.

[33]  Paul S. Rosenbloom,et al.  Towards a New Cognitive Hourglass: Uniform Implementation of Cognitive Architecture via Factor Graphs , 2009 .

[34]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[35]  Antonio Lieto,et al.  Representing Non Classical Concepts in Formal Ontologies: Prototypes and Exemplars , 2011, DART@AI*IA.

[36]  Salvatore Gaglio,et al.  A Cognitive Architecture for Artificial Vision , 1997, Artif. Intell..

[37]  John R. Anderson,et al.  A hybrid model of categorization , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.