Exploring engagement with music in everyday life using experience sampling methodology

Recent qualitative research has highlighted differences in people’s music-listening behaviour according to their level of involvement with and interest in music, yet these findings are mainly based on retrospective accounts of patterns of behaviour (Greasley, 2008; Greasley & Lamont, 2006; Lonie, 2009). Experience sampling methodology (ESM) is a valuable tool for studying music in everyday contexts, and it has been shown to increase people’s conscious awareness of the role of music in their lives (Sloboda, O’Neill, & Ivaldi, 2001). Using ESM, the present study explored differences in people’s everyday engagement with music by recruiting three different types of listener: those identified as having low, moderate, or high engagement with music. Over seven days, quantitative and qualitative data were collected from 25 young adults (aged 18–30) on what they were doing while hearing music (e.g., activities) and the functions/effects of music (e.g., reasons influencing choices). Post-study interviews with 23 participants then generated retrospective accounts about specific musical experiences. Analysis revealed two broad types of listener: the less engaged, who listened for fewer hours a week (min = 3 hours, mean = 12 hours), were less likely to be hearing self-chosen music, and were more likely to listen to music to pass time, out of habit or to help them feel less alone; and the highly engaged, who listened for a greater number of hours per week (mean = 21 hours, max = 40 hours), were more likely to be hearing self-chosen music, and were more likely to use music to evoke specific moods, create an atmosphere, or enhance an activity. The study confirmed the usefulness of ESM for investigating the complex (and interacting) factors involved in people’s daily musical choices, and highlighted ways in which music can fulfil different functions concurrently. Findings show that future research on everyday musical behaviour should continue to account for the context of music listening in shaping responses to and uses of music; and account for individual differences in people’s levels of engagement with music.

[1]  John A. Sloboda,et al.  Everyday Uses of Music Listening: A Preliminary Study , 2004 .

[2]  Harry T. Reis,et al.  Event-sampling and other methods for studying everyday experience. , 2000 .

[3]  Alexandra Lamont,et al.  Music preference in adulthood: Why do we like the music we do? , 2006 .

[4]  J. Sloboda,et al.  Emotions in everyday listening to music. , 2001 .

[5]  M. Csíkszentmihályi Beyond boredom and anxiety , 1975 .

[6]  M. Csíkszentmihályi,et al.  Optimal experience in work and leisure. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  T. DeNora Music in Everyday Life , 2000 .

[8]  P. Laukka,et al.  Expression, Perception, and Induction of Musical Emotions: A Review and a Questionnaire Study of Everyday Listening , 2004 .

[9]  P. Laukka,et al.  Uses of music and psychological well-being among the elderly , 2007 .

[10]  N. Bolger,et al.  Diary methods: capturing life as it is lived. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[11]  Jaakko Erkkilä,et al.  The role of music in adolescents' mood regulation , 2007 .

[12]  A. Lamont young children's musical worlds , 2008 .

[13]  Adrian C. North,et al.  The importance of music to adolescents. , 2000, The British journal of educational psychology.

[14]  Douglas Lonie Musical identities and health over the youth-adult transition , 2009 .

[15]  T. DeNora,et al.  Music and informal learning in everyday life , 2005 .

[16]  E. Guba,et al.  Naturalistic inquiry: Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, 1985, 416 pp., $25.00 (Cloth) , 1985 .

[17]  Rebecca Webb,et al.  Short- and long-term musical preferences: what makes a favourite piece of music? , 2010 .

[18]  Adrian C. North,et al.  Musical preference and taste in childhood and adolescence. , 2006 .

[19]  F. Bailes The prevalence and nature of imagined music in the everyday lives of music students , 2007 .

[20]  Comsumption and Effects of Music in the Media , 2006 .

[21]  J. Sloboda,et al.  The functions of music for affect regulation , 2011 .

[22]  Adrian C. North,et al.  Musical preferences during and after relaxation and exercise. , 2000, The American journal of psychology.

[23]  Joel M. Hektner,et al.  Experience sampling method , 2007 .

[24]  D. Västfjäll,et al.  An experience sampling study of emotional reactions to music: listener, music, and situation. , 2008, Emotion.

[25]  Victor Minichiello,et al.  The meaning of music in the lives of older people: a qualitative study , 2005 .

[26]  Adrian C. North,et al.  Uses of Music in Everyday Life , 2004 .

[27]  Klaus-Ernst Behne,et al.  The development of "Musikerleben" in adolescence: How and why young people listen to music. , 1997 .

[28]  V. Konečni 17 – Social Interaction and Musical Preference , 1982 .

[29]  Daniel Leech-Wilkinson,et al.  European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM) , 1991 .

[30]  J. Sloboda,et al.  Functions of Music in Everyday Life: An Exploratory Study Using the Experience Sampling Method , 2001 .

[31]  유창조 Naturalistic Inquiry , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[32]  Tia DeNora,et al.  Music as a technology of the self , 1999 .

[33]  Adrian C. North,et al.  English and American Adolescents' Reasons for Listening to Music , 2000 .

[34]  Adrian C. North,et al.  The Functions of Music in Everyday Life: Redefining the Social in Music Psychology , 1999 .