Paradoxical extension into the contralesional hemispace in spatial neglect

To explore the idea of a perceptual distortion of space in spatial neglect, neglect patients, age-matched healthy controls and right hemisphere control patients judged the vanishing point of horizontally and vertically-moving stimuli. Hemifield of presentation and movement direction of the stimulus presentation was manipulated. The results suggest that neglect patients show a stronger response bias in the direction of the moving stimuli ("representational momentum") than healthy and right hemisphere controls. Furthermore, neglect patients, but not the control groups, showed a direction-specific response whereby the presence of neglect was associated with a larger representational momentum for leftward-moving stimuli. The one left-hemisphere patient with right-sided neglect showed the opposite effect. Thus, neglect patients showed a relative overextension into their neglected side of space. While these findings are in line with the idea of an extension in the representation of contralesional space, other explanations such as deficient spatial remapping, impairments in smooth pursuit and distortions in memorized visuo-motor movements are considered.

[1]  N. Stucchi,et al.  Visual acceleration and spatial distortion in right brain-damaged patients , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  M. Husain,et al.  Action control in visual neglect , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  3D left hyperschematia after right brain damage , 2008, Neurocase.

[5]  L. Gauthier,et al.  The Bells Test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. , 1989 .

[6]  Paola Guariglia,et al.  Horizontal space misrepresentation in unilateral brain damage II. Eye-head centered modulation of visual misrepresentation in hemianopia without neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  C. Kennard,et al.  Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  Jürgen Konczak,et al.  Kinematics of goal-directed arm movements in neglect: Control of hand in space , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  G. Kerkhoff Multiple perceptual distortions and their modulation in leftsided visual neglect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Spatial Compression in Visual Neglect: A Case Study , 1991, Cortex.

[11]  R A Finke,et al.  Facilitation of length discrimination using real and imaged context frames. , 1984, The American journal of psychology.

[12]  P. McGeorge,et al.  Representing target motion: the role of the right hemisphere in the forward displacement bias. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[13]  S. Ferber,et al.  How to Assess Spatial Neglect - Line Bisection or Cancellation Tasks? , 2001, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[14]  Peter Brugger,et al.  Random number generation in neglect patients reveals enhanced response stereotypy, but no neglect in number space , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Ian M. Thornton,et al.  Representational momentum: New findings, new directions , 2002 .

[16]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Attention maintains mental extrapolation of target position: irrelevant distractors eliminate forward displacement after implied motion , 2003, Cognition.

[17]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Vestibulo-ocular and optokinetic impairments in left unilateral neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Two-dimensional distortion of space representation in unilateral neglect: perceptual and response-related factors , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  R. Yee,et al.  Optokinetic nystagmus and parietal lobe lesions , 1980, Annals of neurology.

[20]  Keith Muir,et al.  Immediate and delayed reaching in hemispatial neglect , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Beyond unilateral neglect. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  K. Heilman,et al.  Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.

[23]  Hans-Otto Karnath,et al.  Goal-Directed Hand Movements Are Not Affected by the Biased Space Representation in Spatial Neglect , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.

[25]  Edoardo Bisiach,et al.  Visual Awareness and Anisometry of Space Representation in Unilateral Neglect: A Panoramic Investigation by Means of a Line Extension Task , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[26]  K. Heilman,et al.  Right hemisphere dominance for attention , 1980, Neurology.

[27]  F. Doricchi,et al.  Misrepresentation of horizontal space in left unilateral neglect , 1999, Neurology.

[28]  P. Malhotra,et al.  The neural basis of visuomotor deficits in hemispatial neglect , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Fabrizio Doricchi,et al.  Horizontal space misrepresentation in unilateral brain damage I. Visual and proprioceptive-motor influences in left unilateral neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  G. Iaria,et al.  The “ways” we look at dreams: evidence from unilateral spatial neglect (with an evolutionary account of dream bizarreness) , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Impaired Perceptual Memory of Locations across Gaze-shifts in Patients with Unilateral Spatial Neglect , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  G. Vallar,et al.  When the whole is more than the sum of the parts: evidence from visuospatial neglect. , 2008, Journal of neuropsychology.

[33]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect , 2002 .

[35]  Edoardo Bisiach,et al.  Challenging current accounts of unilateral neglect , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  D. Kerzel Eye movements and visible persistence explain the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target , 2000, Vision Research.

[37]  S. Ferber,et al.  Is space representation distorted in neglect? , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Impaired Working Memory for Location but not for Colour or Shape in Visual Neglect: a Comparison of Parietal and Non-Parietal Lesions , 2004, Cortex.

[39]  F. Doricchi,et al.  Amplitude and speed change of the optokinetic response in patients with and without neglect. , 1995, Neuroreport.

[40]  J. Lynch,et al.  Optokinetic nystagmus deficits following parieto-occipital cortex lesions in monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Lack of Impairments on Leftward and Rightward Line Extension Tasks in Neglect Patients , 2000, The International journal of neuroscience.

[42]  Jennifer J. Freyd,et al.  Representational momentum when attention is divided , 2002 .

[43]  T. Hubbard Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  A. Cowey,et al.  A distortion of perceived space in patients withright-hemisphere lesions and visual hemineglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  M. Sugishita,et al.  Improvement of left unilateral spatial neglect in a line extension task , 1994, Neurology.

[46]  A. Milner,et al.  Distortion of size perception in visuospatial neglect , 1995, Current Biology.

[47]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[48]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Left size distortion (hyperschematia) after right brain damage , 2006, Neurology.

[49]  M. Alberoni,et al.  Direction and position factors in performance of line extension task by unilateral neglect subjects , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  Jenni A Ogden,et al.  Anterior-posterior interhemispheric differences in the loci of lesions producing visual hemineglect , 1985, Brain and Cognition.

[51]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[52]  Kenneth M. Heilman,et al.  Directional hypokinesia , 1985, Neurology.

[53]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  The role of perception in the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  J-M Beis,et al.  Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[55]  A. Chatterjee Spatial anisometry and representational release in neglect , 2002 .

[56]  G. Galati,et al.  The endpoints’ task: an analysis of length reproduction in unilateral neglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.