Advancement of motion psychophysics: review 2001-2010.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Zijiang J. He,et al. Boundary contour-based surface integration affected by color , 2010, Vision Research.
[2] Shaul Hochstein,et al. Isolating the effect of one-dimensional motion signals on the perceived direction of moving two-dimensional objects , 1993, Vision Research.
[3] R. Sekuler,et al. Hysteresis in the perception of motion direction as evidence for neural cooperativity , 1986, Nature.
[4] Roberto Arrighi,et al. Meaningful auditory information enhances perception of visual biological motion. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[5] John A. Greenwood,et al. Pushing the limits of transparent-motion detection with binocular disparity , 2006, Vision Research.
[6] Robert F. Hess,et al. Second-order optic flow processing , 2007, Vision Research.
[7] F. A. Miles,et al. Reversed short-latency ocular following , 2002, Vision Research.
[8] Kazushi Maruya,et al. Adaptation to invisible motion results in low-level but not high-level aftereffects. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[9] R. Snowden,et al. Shifts in perceived position following adaptation to visual motion , 1998, Current Biology.
[10] Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al. Smooth shifts of visual attention , 2002, Vision Research.
[11] Brian J Scholl,et al. “Perceptual Scotomas” , 2008, Psychological science.
[12] Peter Wenderoth,et al. The different mechanisms of the motion direction illusion and aftereffect , 2007, Vision Research.
[13] Jane H Sumnall,et al. The extra-retinal motion aftereffect. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[14] S. Dakin,et al. Snakes and ladders: the role of temporal modulation in visual contour integration , 2001, Vision Research.
[15] Marco Bertamini,et al. Illusory surfaces affect the integration of local motion signals , 2004, Vision Research.
[16] Qin Hu,et al. A set of high-order spatiotemporal stimuli that elicit motion and reverse-phi percepts. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[17] Bart Farell,et al. A new theory of structure-from-motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[18] P. Verghese,et al. Combining speed information across space , 1995, Vision Research.
[19] D. Purves,et al. The wagon wheel illusion in movies and reality. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[20] J. Movshon,et al. Integration of sensory evidence in motion discrimination. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[21] Christopher C. Pack,et al. The role of V1 surround suppression in MT motion integration. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] P. Tse,et al. Mechanisms underlying the perceived angular velocity of a rigidly rotating object , 2006, Vision Research.
[23] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Stimulus magnification equates identification and discrimination of biological motion across the visual field , 2008, Vision Research.
[24] R. Blake,et al. What constitutes an efficient reference frame for vision? , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] A. Doorn,et al. Spatio-temporal tuning of motion coherence detection at different luminance levels , 2002, Vision Research.
[26] Marijn C. W. Kroes,et al. The parallel between reverse-phi and motion aftereffects. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[27] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. How motion signals are integrated across frequencies: study on motion perception and ocular following responses using multiple-slit stimuli. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[28] Brian A Wandell,et al. Assessment of stimulus-induced changes in human V1 visual field maps. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[29] John Schlag,et al. A new form of illusory conjunction between color and shape , 2010 .
[30] Kenji Yokoi,et al. Dynamic distortion of visual position representation around moving objects. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[31] Marijn C. W. Kroes,et al. Sensitivity for reverse-phi motion , 2009, Vision Research.
[32] B. Cumming,et al. All Pulfrich-like illusions can be explained without joint encoding of motion and disparity. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[33] Peter Wenderoth,et al. Dichoptic reduction of the direction illusion is not due to binocular rivalry , 2010, Vision Research.
[34] Dirk Kerzel,et al. The trial context determines adjusted localization of stimuli: reconciling the Fröhlich and onset repulsion effects , 2004, Vision Research.
[35] David C. Burr,et al. Pooling and segmenting motion signals , 2009, Vision Research.
[36] John Harris,et al. Spatial Offset of Test Field Elements from Surround Elements Affects the Strength of Motion Aftereffects , 2008, Perception.
[37] Y. Ejima,et al. Dependencies of Motion Assimilation and Motion Contrast on Spatial Properties of Stimuli: Spatial-frequency Nonselective and Selective Interactions Between Local Motion Detectors , 1997, Vision Research.
[38] R. Wurtz,et al. An illusory transformation of optic flow fields , 1993, Vision Research.
[39] David R. Badcock,et al. Second-order orientation cues to the axis of motion , 2009, Vision Research.
[40] William H Warren,et al. Catching fly balls in virtual reality: a critical test of the outfielder problem. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[41] R. Gottsdanker. The ability of human operators to detect acceleration of target motion. , 1956, Psychological bulletin.
[42] David R. Badcock,et al. Detecting the displacement of periodic patterns , 1985, Vision Research.
[43] R O Duncan,et al. Occlusion and the Interpretation of Visual Motion: Perceptual and Neuronal Effects of Context , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Mobile computation: spatiotemporal integration of the properties of objects in motion. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[45] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Interattribute apparent motion , 1989, Vision Research.
[46] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. A motion aftereffect seen more strongly by the non-adapted eye: evidence of multistage adaptation in visual motion processing , 2001, Vision Research.
[47] T Ledgeway,et al. The Duration of the Motion Aftereffect following Adaptation to First-Order and Second-Order Motion , 1994, Perception.
[48] I. Murakami. Correlations between fixation stability and visual motion sensitivity , 2004, Vision Research.
[49] Fang Fang,et al. Strong influence of test patterns on the perception of motion aftereffect and position. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[50] Randolph Blake,et al. Weakened Center-Surround Interactions in Visual Motion Processing in Schizophrenia , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[51] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Characterizing global and local mechanisms in biological motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[52] Kathy T. Mullen,et al. S-cone contributions to linear and non-linear motion processing , 2007, Vision Research.
[53] Derek H. Arnold,et al. Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression , 2009, Current Biology.
[54] R Nijhawan,et al. The Flash-Lag Phenomenon: Object Motion and Eye Movements , 2001, Perception.
[55] S. Zeki,et al. The disunity of consciousness , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[56] D. Alais,et al. Tilt aftereffects and tilt illusions induced by fast translational motion: evidence for motion streaks. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[57] S. Nishida,et al. A common perceptual temporal limit of binding synchronous inputs across different sensory attributes and modalities , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[58] T. Ledgeway,et al. The influence of spatial and temporal noise on the detection of first-order and second-order orientation and motion direction , 2005, Vision Research.
[59] Joan López-Moliner,et al. Perceptual asynchrony between color and motion with a single direction change. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[60] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Visual jitter: evidence for visual-motion-based compensation of retinal slip due to small eye movements , 2001, Vision Research.
[61] Peter W. McOwan,et al. Robust velocity computation from a biologically motivated model of motion perception , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[62] Leo Poom,et al. Colour, Polarity, Disparity, and Texture Contributions to Motion Segregation , 2005, Perception.
[63] Allison M McKendrick,et al. Pattern cues disambiguate perceived direction in simple moving stimuli , 2003, Vision Research.
[64] Akihiro Yagi,et al. Biological Motion Alters Coherent Motion Perception , 2008, Perception.
[65] A. T. Smith,et al. Antagonistic comparison of temporal frequency filter outputs as a basis for speed perception , 1994, Vision Research.
[66] O. Braddick,et al. Speed and direction of locally-paired dot patterns , 2000, Vision Research.
[67] Ikuya Murakami,et al. Functional brain imaging of the Rotating Snakes illusion by fMRI. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[68] Norimichi Kitagawa,et al. Hearing visual motion in depth , 2002, Nature.
[69] John Harris,et al. Perceived spatial displacement of motion-defined contours in peripheral vision , 2008, Vision Research.
[70] M. Georgeson,et al. Motion blur and motion sharpening: temporal smear and local contrast non-linearity , 1998, Vision Research.
[71] S. Grossberg,et al. Laminar cortical dynamics of visual form and motion interactions during coherent object motion perception. , 2007, Spatial vision.
[72] G. Sperling. Movement perception in computer-driven visual displays , 1976 .
[73] Bart Krekelberg,et al. Summation of Visual Motion across Eye Movements Reflects a Nonspatial Decision Mechanism , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[74] D. Burr,et al. Temporal integration of optic flow, measured by contrast and coherence thresholds , 2001, Vision Research.
[75] D. Dennett,et al. The Nature of Consciousness , 2006 .
[76] A J Ahumada,et al. Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[77] George Mather,et al. Psychophysical properties of two-stroke apparent motion. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[78] Neil W. Roach,et al. Centrifugal propagation of motion adaptation effects across visual space. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[79] M. Ibbotson,et al. Neurons in V1, V2, and PMLS of cat cortex are speed tuned but not acceleration tuned: the influence of motion adaptation. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[80] K. Gegenfurtner,et al. Motion perception at scotopic light levels. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[81] B. Cumming,et al. The stroboscopic Pulfrich effect is not evidence for the joint encoding of motion and depth. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[82] S. Klein,et al. Opponent-movement mechanisms in human vision. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[83] Erik Blaser,et al. When is Motion ‘Motion’? , 2008, Perception.
[84] Erich W. Graf,et al. Contextual effects in speed perception may occur at an early stage of processing , 2010, Vision Research.
[85] Derek H. Arnold,et al. Motion-induced spatial conflict , 2003, Nature.
[86] Colin W G Clifford,et al. Determinants of asynchronous processing in vision , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[87] J. Enns,et al. Object Updating and the Flash-Lag Effect , 2004, Psychological science.
[88] M. Georgeson,et al. Seeing blur: ‘motion sharpenin’ without motion , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[89] Thomas A Carlson,et al. Floating square illusion: perceptual uncoupling of static and dynamic objects in motion. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[90] Alan L. F. Lee,et al. A comparison of global motion perception using a multiple-aperture stimulus. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[91] Paul V McGraw,et al. Motion-Sensitive Neurones in V5/MT Modulate Perceived Spatial Position , 2004, Current Biology.
[92] Matthew C Smear,et al. Perception of Fourier and non-Fourier motion by larval zebrafish , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[93] Claire V Hutchinson,et al. Asymmetric spatial frequency tuning of motion mechanisms in human vision revealed by masking. , 2007, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.
[94] S. Aghdaee,et al. Adaptation to Spiral Motion in Crowding Condition , 2005, Perception.
[95] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Acceleration carries the local inversion effect in biological motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[96] P. Cavanagh,et al. Position displacement, not velocity, is the cue to motion detection of second-order stimuli , 1998, Vision Research.
[97] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Apparent speed increases at low luminance. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[98] Craig Aaen-Stockdale,et al. Global motion processing: The effect of spatial scale and eccentricity. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[99] Joseph Krummenacher,et al. A (fascinating) litmus test for human retino- vs. non-retinotopic processing. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[100] R A Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. I. Psychophysics , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[101] G. Alvarez. Representing multiple objects as an ensemble enhances visual cognition , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[102] S. Dakin,et al. Comparison of the spatial-frequency selectivity of local and global motion detectors. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[103] J. Lappin,et al. Coherence of early motion signals , 2001, Vision Research.
[104] Romi Nijhawan,et al. Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.
[105] A. Derrington,et al. Separate detectors for simple and complex grating patterns? , 1985, Vision Research.
[106] P. Cavanagh,et al. Independent, synchronous access to color and motion features , 2008, Cognition.
[107] David R. Badcock,et al. Analysis of the motion of 2-dimensional patterns: Evidence for a second-order process , 1992, Vision Research.
[108] Randolph Blake,et al. Strength of early visual adaptation depends on visual awareness , 2006 .
[109] D. Badcock,et al. Global-motion Perception: Interaction of Chromatic and Luminance Signals , 1996, Vision Research.
[110] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Contrast-reversing global-motion stimuli reveal local interactions between first- and second-order motion signals , 2004, Vision Research.
[111] Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al. Motion in depth based on inter-ocular velocity differences , 2000, Vision Research.
[112] Christopher P Benton,et al. Fractal rotation isolates mechanisms for form-dependent motion in human vision , 2007, Biology Letters.
[113] P. Cavanagh,et al. Visual stability based on remapping of attention pointers , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[114] Neil W. Roach,et al. Report a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern , 2016 .
[115] Szonya Durant,et al. Latency differences and the flash-lag effect , 2003, Vision Research.
[116] C. Clifford. Perceptual adaptation: motion parallels orientation , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[117] C Busettini,et al. Short-latency ocular following in humans: sensitivity to binocular disparity , 2001, Vision Research.
[118] Ignacio Serrano-Pedraza,et al. Antagonism between fine and coarse motion sensors depends on stimulus size and contrast. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[119] Norberto M. Grzywacz,et al. Parametric decomposition of optic flow by humans , 2005, Vision Research.
[120] Y. Ejima,et al. Estimation of the Timing of Human Visual Perception from Magnetoencephalography , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[121] Timothy Ledgeway,et al. Cortical pooling algorithms for judging global motion direction , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[122] DAVID ALAIS,et al. The Size and Number of Plaid Blobs Mediate the Misperception of Type-II Plaid Direction , 1997, Vision Research.
[123] P. Cavanagh,et al. Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.
[124] Peter W. McOwan,et al. Computational modelling of interleaved first- and second-order motion sequences and translating 3f+4f beat patterns , 2000, Vision Research.
[125] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Peripheral sensitivity to biological motion conveyed by first and second-order signals , 2010, Vision Research.
[126] Allan J. Pantle,et al. Direction-specific changes of sensitivity after brief apparent motion stimuli , 2001, Vision Research.
[127] R. Hess,et al. Low-level mechanisms may contribute to paradoxical motion percepts. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[128] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. The Audiovisual Tau Effect in Infancy , 2010, PloS one.
[129] Alan Johnston,et al. Motion drag induced by global motion Gabor arrays. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[130] A. Johnston,et al. Categorizing sex and identity from the biological motion of faces , 2001, Current Biology.
[131] P. McOwan,et al. Perception of motion direction in luminance-and contrast-defined reversed-phi motion sequences , 1997, Vision Research.
[132] David Whitney,et al. The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[133] Michael S. Landy,et al. The kinetic depth effect and optic flowȁII. First- and second-order motion , 1991, Vision Research.
[134] J. Krauskopf,et al. Influence of colour on the perception of coherent motion , 1990, Nature.
[135] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Frames of reference for biological motion and face perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[136] Su-Ling Yeh,et al. A common mechanism for perceptual filling-in and motion-induced blindness , 2006, Vision Research.
[137] Nicholas J. Priebe,et al. The Neural Representation of Speed in Macaque Area MT/V5 , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[138] Emily D. Grossman,et al. Necessary but not sufficient: Motion perception is required for perceiving biological motion , 2008, Vision Research.
[139] P. Werkhoven,et al. Visual processing of optic acceleration , 1992, Vision Research.
[140] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[141] Jennifer L. Campos,et al. Bayesian integration of visual and vestibular signals for heading. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[142] David Whitney,et al. Motion distorts perceived position without awareness of motion , 2005, Current Biology.
[143] Takeo Watanabe,et al. Neuroimaging of direction-selective mechanisms for second-order motion. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[144] J. Gibson. On the analysis of change in the optic array. , 1977, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[145] S. Nishida,et al. Adaptive pooling of visual motion signals by the human visual system revealed with a novel multi-element stimulus. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[146] Jochen Ditterich. Stochastic models of decisions about motion direction: behavior and physiology , 2006 .
[147] M. Shiffrar,et al. Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines , 1993, Vision Research.
[148] F. A. Miles,et al. Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.
[149] Andrei Gorea,et al. Sensory and decisional factors in motion-induced blindness. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[150] David C. Burr,et al. Cardinal axes for radial and circular motion, revealed by summation and by masking , 2001, Vision Research.
[151] Bruce Luber,et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation differentially affects speed and direction judgments , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[152] Tatsuto Takeuchi,et al. Velocity discrimination in scotopic vision , 2000, Vision Research.
[153] Hongjing Lu,et al. Structural processing in biological motion perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[154] Joachim Hohnsbein,et al. Visual Analysis of Changes of Motion in Reaction-Time Tasks , 2005, Perception.
[155] David Whitney,et al. Position shifts following crowded second-order motion adaptation reveal processing of local and global motion without awareness. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[156] Jason Bell,et al. Local motion effects on form in radial frequency patterns. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[157] Ning Qian,et al. Motion rivalry impairs motion repulsion , 2001, Vision Research.
[158] Sophie M. Wuerger,et al. Reaction time facilitation for horizontally moving auditory-visual stimuli. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[159] Robert F Hess,et al. Global motion processing: invariance with mean luminance. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[160] D. Lindsey. Direction repulsion in unfiltered and ring-filtered Julesz textures , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[161] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. A model of neuronal responses in visual area MT , 1998, Vision Research.
[162] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Visual motion aftereffects arise from a cascade of two isomorphic adaptation mechanisms. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[163] Lee A. Gilroy,et al. A common mechanism for the perception of first-order and second-order apparent motion , 2005, Vision Research.
[164] A. Pinkus,et al. Probing Visual Motion Signals with a Priming Paradigm , 1997, Vision Research.
[165] Junji Watanabe,et al. Veridical perception of moving colors by trajectory integration of input signals. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[166] R. Sekuler,et al. Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.
[167] David R. Badcock,et al. Global motion perception: Interaction of the ON and OFF pathways , 1994, Vision Research.
[168] David Alais,et al. Neural latencies do not explain the auditory and audio-visual flash-lag effect , 2005, Vision Research.
[169] Qasim Zaidi,et al. Visual processing of motion boundaries , 1995, Vision Research.
[170] Katsumi Watanabe,et al. The motion-induced position shift depends on the visual awareness of motion , 2005, Vision Research.
[171] Dora E. Angelaki,et al. Sensory Convergence Solves a Motion Ambiguity Problem , 2005, Current Biology.
[172] Kazushi Maruya,et al. Spatial pooling of one-dimensional second-order motion signals. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[173] T. Freeman,et al. Human sensitivity to expanding and rotating motion: effects of complementary masking and directional structure , 1992, Vision Research.
[174] Johannes M. Zanker,et al. Theta motion: a paradoxical stimulus to explore higher order motion extraction , 1993, Vision Research.
[175] Joan López-Moliner,et al. Vision affects how fast we hear sounds move. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[176] Tatsuto Takeuchi,et al. The effect of retinal illuminance on visual motion priming , 2011, Vision Research.
[177] L. Bowns. Evidence for a Feature Tracking Explanation of Why Type II Plaids Move in the Vector Sum Direction at Short Durations , 1996, Vision Research.
[178] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Perceptual-binding and persistent surface segregation , 2004, Vision Research.
[179] Alan C. Evans,et al. Cortical specialization for processing first- and second-order motion. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[180] P. Verghese,et al. Spatial Layout Affects Speed Discrimination , 1997, Vision Research.
[181] Bart Krekelberg,et al. Effects of spatial attention and salience cues on chromatic and achromatic motion processing , 2007, Vision Research.
[182] Gianluca Campana,et al. Implied motion from static photographs influences the perceived position of stationary objects , 2011, Vision Research.
[183] Christopher C. Pack,et al. Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.
[184] Timothy Ledgeway,et al. Spatial summation of first-order and second-order motion in human vision , 2010, Vision Research.
[185] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Subjective disappearance of a target by flickering flankers , 2007, Vision Research.
[186] M. Lappe,et al. Perception of biological motion from limited-lifetime stimuli , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[187] Hersh Sagreiya,et al. Explaining the footsteps, belly dancer, Wenceslas, and kickback illusions. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[188] Linda Bowns,et al. ‘Squaring’ is Better at Predicting Plaid Motion than the Vector Average or Intersection of Constraints , 2006, Perception.
[189] Markus Huff,et al. Conflicting motion information impairs multiple object tracking. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[190] Daniel Linares,et al. Position perception: influence of motion with displacement dissociated from the influence of motion alone. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[191] Franco Lepore,et al. Role of primary visual cortex in the binocular integration of plaid motion perception , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[192] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Form constraints in motion binding , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[193] Stephane J. M. Rainville,et al. Opponent-motion mechanisms are self-normalizing , 2005, Vision Research.
[194] William Curran,et al. Direction Repulsion Goes Global , 2003, Current Biology.
[195] N. Troje,et al. The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.
[196] Minami Ito,et al. Representation of Angles Embedded within Contour Stimuli in Area V2 of Macaque Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[197] P. Verghese,et al. Integration of speed signals in the direction of motion , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[198] Iain D Gilchrist,et al. The target velocity integration function for saccades. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[199] Christopher P. Benton,et al. A role for contrast-normalisation in second-order motion perception , 2004, Vision Research.
[200] J. Lange,et al. Visual perception of biological motion by form: a template-matching analysis. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[201] S. Nishida. Motion-Based Analysis of Spatial Patterns by the Human Visual System , 2004, Current Biology.
[202] S. Anstis. The perception of apparent movement. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[203] D. Eagleman,et al. Illusory motion reversal is caused by rivalry, not by perceptual snapshots of the visual field , 2004, Vision Research.
[204] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. A new transparent motion aftereffect , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[205] Tatjana Seizova-Cajic,et al. Illusory motion reversals from unambiguous motion with visual, proprioceptive, and tactile stimuli , 2008, Vision Research.
[206] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. A jitter after-effect reveals motion-based stabilization of vision , 1998, Nature.
[207] Randolph Blake,et al. Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor , 2005, Vision Research.
[208] M. Thirkettle,et al. Contributions of form, motion and task to biological motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[209] C. Koch,et al. Attention-driven discrete sampling of motion perception. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[210] Sieu K. Khuu,et al. Global speed processing: evidence for local averaging within, but not across two speed ranges , 2002, Vision Research.
[211] F. Pollick,et al. Recognising Facial Expression from Spatially and Temporally Modified Movements , 2003, Perception.
[212] J. Movshon,et al. A new perceptual illusion reveals mechanisms of sensory decoding , 2007, Nature.
[213] Avi Chaudhuri,et al. Eye movements and the motion aftereffect: Alternatives to the induced motion hypothesis , 1991, Vision Research.
[214] E H Adelson,et al. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[215] S. Nishida,et al. Perceptual ambiguity of bistable visual stimuli causes no or little increase in perceptual latency. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[216] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Large-Field Visual Motion Directly Induces an Involuntary Rapid Manual Following Response , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[217] Hugh R. Wilson,et al. Global shape coding for motion-defined radial-frequency contours , 2005, Vision Research.
[218] V. Ramachandran,et al. Motion capture anisotropy , 1987, Vision Research.
[219] Allison B Sekuler,et al. Spatial characteristics of center-surround antagonism in younger and older adults. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[220] Su-Ling Yeh,et al. Linking motion-induced blindness to perceptual filling-in , 2004, Vision Research.
[221] Harold E. Bedell,et al. Spatial and temporal properties of the illusory motion-induced position shift for drifting stimuli , 2007, Vision Research.
[222] Thomas U. Otto,et al. Assessing the microstructure of motion correspondences with non-retinotopic feature attribution. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[223] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Dynamics of neuronal responses in macaque MT and VIP during motion detection , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[224] Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al. Motion processing at low light levels: Differential effects on the perception of specific motion types. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[225] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Early binding of feature pairs for visual perception , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[226] Hinze Hogendoorn,et al. Interpolation and extrapolation on the path of apparent motion , 2008, Vision Research.
[227] A. Montagnini,et al. Do we track what we see? Common versus independent processing for motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements: A review , 2011, Vision Research.
[228] Luis A. Lesmes,et al. The mechanism of isoluminant chromatic motion perception. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[229] Mary M. Conte,et al. Nonlinear Preprocessing in Short-range Motion , 1997, Vision Research.
[230] Mark Edwards,et al. Independence in the processing of first- and second-order motion signals at the local-motion-pooling level , 2010, Vision Research.
[231] R. Blake,et al. Motion Perception Getting Better with Age? , 2005, Neuron.
[232] A. Derrington,et al. Motion of contrast-modulated gratings is analysed by different mechanisms at low and at high contrasts , 2000, Vision Research.
[233] V. S. Ramachandran,et al. Extrapolation of motion path in human visual perception , 1983, Vision Research.
[234] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Perceiving a discontinuity in motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[235] M. J. M. Lankheet,et al. PII: S0042-6989(00)00187-5 , 2000 .
[236] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Direction repulsion between components in motion transparency , 1996, Vision Research.
[237] Norberto M. Grzywacz,et al. Measurement of rate of expansion in the perception of radial motion , 2005, Vision Research.
[238] P. Kornprobst,et al. Modelling the dynamics of motion integration with a new luminance-gated diffusion mechanism , 2010, Vision Research.
[239] Kathy T Mullen,et al. The Magnocellular visual pathway and the flash-lag illusion. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[240] Jon Driver,et al. Direction of Visual Apparent Motion Driven Solely by Timing of a Static Sound , 2008, Current Biology.
[241] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Limits of attentive tracking reveal temporal properties of attention , 2000, Vision Research.
[242] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Audiovisual tau effect. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[243] T. Albright,et al. Image Segmentation Enhances Discrimination of Motion in Visual Noise , 1997, Vision Research.
[244] Derek H. Arnold,et al. Asynchronous processing in vision Color leads motion , 2001, Current Biology.
[245] Markus Lappe,et al. Absolute travel distance from optic flow , 2005, Vision Research.
[246] Eric Hiris,et al. Detection of biological and nonbiological motion. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[247] Mary M. Conte,et al. Coherence and transparency of moving plaids composed of Fourier and non-Fourier gratings , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[248] Erich W Graf,et al. Motion-aftereffect-induced blindness. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[249] G. Mather,et al. Low-level visual processing of biological motion , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[250] David C. Burr,et al. Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.
[251] Ryota Kanai,et al. Dynamical evolution of motion perception , 2007, Vision Research.
[252] Timothy Ledgeway,et al. Spatial frequency selective masking of first-order and second-order motion in the absence of off-frequency `looking' , 2004, Vision Research.
[253] Kazushi Maruya,et al. Conditional spatial-frequency selective pooling of one-dimensional motion signals into global two-dimensional motion , 2010, Vision Research.
[254] Edward H Adelson,et al. Junctions and cost functions in motion interpretation. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[255] R A Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. III. Modeling , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[256] P. Viviani,et al. Colour, form, and movement are not perceived simultaneously , 2001, Vision Research.
[257] Reza Farivar,et al. Co-operative interactions between first- and second-order mechanisms in the processing of structure from motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[258] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Active versus passive processing of biological motion , 2002, Perception.
[259] K. Nakayama,et al. Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion , 1989, Vision Research.
[260] Satoshi Shioiri,et al. Adaptation to relative and uniform motion. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[261] A. Hayes,et al. Apparent position governs contour–element binding by the visual system , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[262] Joan López-Moliner,et al. Motion signal and the perceived positions of moving objects. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[263] Izumi Ohzawa,et al. Joint-encoding of motion and depth by visual cortical neurons: neural basis of the Pulfrich effect , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[264] M. Shadlen,et al. Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[265] Stephen T. Hammett,et al. Speed can go up as well as down at low contrast: Implications for models of motion perception , 2006, Vision Research.
[266] Andrew M. Derrington,et al. Errors in direction-of-motion discrimination with complex stimuli , 1987, Vision Research.
[267] B. Krekelberg,et al. Motion Detection Mechanisms , 2008 .
[268] Ikuya Murakami,et al. Illusory position shift induced by cyclopean motion , 2009, Vision Research.
[269] Hyung-Chul O. Li,et al. Segregation by color/luminance does not necessarily facilitate motion discrimination in the presence of motion distractors , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[270] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Center–surround inhibition deepens binocular rivalry suppression , 2005, Vision Research.
[271] B J Rogers,et al. Illusory Continuous Motion from Oscillating Positive-Negative Patterns: Implications for Motion Perception , 1986, Perception.
[272] Yong Gu,et al. Decoding of MSTd Population Activity Accounts for Variations in the Precision of Heading Perception , 2010, Neuron.
[273] Ian E Holliday,et al. Optic flow in human vision: MEG reveals a foveo-fugal bias in V1, specialization for spiral space in hMSTs, and global motion sensitivity in the IPS. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[274] Michael J Morgan,et al. Linear mechanisms can produce motion sharpening , 2001, Vision Research.
[275] Antony B. Morland,et al. Perceptual distortions of speed at low luminance: Evidence inconsistent with a Bayesian account of speed encoding , 2007, Vision Research.
[276] S. Nishida,et al. Spatial-frequency tuning in the pooling of one- and two-dimensional motion signals , 2009, Vision Research.
[277] S. Nishida,et al. Two mechanisms underlying the effect of angle of motion direction change on colour–motion asynchrony , 2007, Vision Research.
[278] Robert Sekuler,et al. Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.
[279] Andrew T. Smith,et al. The Representation of Egomotion in the Human Brain , 2008, Current Biology.
[280] R. Blake,et al. Another means for measuring the motion aftereffect , 1993, Vision Research.
[281] David C. Burr,et al. Seeing biological motion , 1998, Nature.
[282] Leland S Stone,et al. Spatial scale of stereomotion speed processing. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[283] R. Snowden,et al. Phantom motion aftereffects – evidence of detectors for the analysis of optic flow , 1997, Current Biology.
[284] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Influence of viewing distance on aftereffects of moving random pixel arrays , 2003, Vision Research.
[285] S. Nishida,et al. Marker Correspondence, Not Processing Latency, Determines Temporal Binding of Visual Attributes , 2002, Current Biology.
[286] Philippe Lefèvre,et al. Biological motion influences the visuomotor transformation for smooth pursuit eye movements , 2010, Vision Research.
[287] Leland S Stone,et al. Stereomotion speed perception: contributions from both changing disparity and interocular velocity difference over a range of relative disparities. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[288] I. Murakami,et al. A positive correlation between fixation instability and the strength of illusory motion in a static display , 2006, Vision Research.
[289] Timothy J. Andrews,et al. The wheels keep turning Reply to Holcombe et al. , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[290] S S Shimozaki,et al. The maintenance of apparent luminance of an object. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[291] R. Wurtz,et al. Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.
[292] David R. Badcock,et al. Detecting the displacements of spatial beats: No role for distortion products , 1989, Vision Research.
[293] Bas Rokers,et al. Strong percepts of motion through depth without strong percepts of position in depth. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[294] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Perceptual manifestations of fast neural plasticity: Motion priming, rapid motion aftereffect and perceptual sensitization , 2005, Vision Research.
[295] Allison M. McKendrick,et al. Aging alters surround modulation of perceived contrast. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[296] C. Colby,et al. Trans-saccadic perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[297] David Whitaker,et al. Motion Adaptation Distorts Perceived Visual Position , 2002, Current Biology.
[298] Derek H. Arnold,et al. Motion induced spatial conflict following binocular integration , 2005, Vision Research.
[299] Sieu K. Khuu,et al. The perceived position shift of a pattern that contains internal motion is accompanied by a change in the pattern’s apparent size and shape , 2007, Vision Research.
[300] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Changing objects lead briefly flashed ones , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[301] Randolph Blake,et al. Contextual modulations of center-surround interactions in motion revealed with the motion aftereffect. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[302] Andrei Gorea,et al. Adaptation and prolonged inhibition as a main cause of motion-induced blindness. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[303] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. The Motion Aftereffect:A Modern Perspective , 1998 .
[304] S. Zeki,et al. Toward a Theory of Visual Consciousness , 1999, Consciousness and Cognition.
[305] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Spatiotemporal feature attribution for the perception of visual size. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[306] Najib J Majaj,et al. Binocular Integration of Pattern Motion Signals by MT Neurons and by Human Observers , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[307] Constance S. Royden,et al. A model using MT-like motion-opponent operators explains an illusory transformation in the optic flow field , 2003, Vision Research.
[308] V S Ramachandran,et al. Sharpness Constancy during Movement Perception (Short Note) , 1974, Perception.
[309] T. Poggio,et al. Visual hyperacuity: spatiotemporal interpolation in human vision , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[310] Erich W Graf,et al. Equivalence of physical and perceived speed in binocular rivalry. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[311] Stephen J Heinen,et al. Perceptual and oculomotor evidence of limitations on processing accelerating motion. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[312] Neil W. Roach,et al. Dynamics of Spatial Distortions Reveal Multiple Time Scales of Motion Adaptation , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[313] R. Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[314] Kathy T Mullen,et al. The contribution of color to global motion processing. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[315] Dario L. Ringach,et al. Flies see second-order motion , 2008, Current Biology.
[316] Andrew M. Derrington,et al. The analysis of motion of two-dimensional patterns: do Fourier components provide the first stage? , 1994, Vision Research.
[317] Ennio Mingolla,et al. Neural models of motion integration and segmentation , 2003, Neural Networks.
[318] Bevil R. Conway,et al. Spatiotemporal Structure of Nonlinear Subunits in Macaque Visual Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[319] Rebecca A. Champion,et al. A ratio model of perceived speed in the human visual system , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[320] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Correlated changes in perceptions of the gender and orientation of ambiguous biological motion figures , 2008, Current Biology.
[321] P Cavanagh,et al. A Moving Display Which Opposes Short-Range and Long-Range Signals , 1985, Perception.
[322] Anna Brooks,et al. An illusion of coherent global motion arising from single brief presentations of a stationary stimulus , 2003, Vision Research.
[323] Dov Sagi,et al. Motion-induced blindness in normal observers , 2001, Nature.
[324] Neil W. Roach,et al. Distortions of perceived auditory and visual space following adaptation to motion , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[325] David R. Badcock,et al. Detecting the displacements of spatial beats: A monocular capability , 1987, Vision Research.
[326] Eric Castet,et al. Parallel Motion Processing for the Initiation of Short-Latency Ocular Following in Humans , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[327] Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al. Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.
[328] D Kerzel,et al. Time-to-passage judgments on circular trajectories are based on relative optical acceleration , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[329] Paul B Hibbard,et al. Global motion processing is not tuned for binocular disparity , 1999, Vision Research.
[330] Christof Koch,et al. Modeling Reverse-Phi Motion-Selective Neurons in Cortex: Double Synaptic-Veto Mechanism , 2003, Neural Computation.
[331] Anthony J. Movshon,et al. Optimal representation of sensory information by neural populations , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[332] S. Dakin,et al. The role of relative motion computation in ‘direction repulsion’ , 2000, Vision Research.
[333] Karen K. De Valois,et al. Visual motion mechanisms under low retinal illuminance revealed by motion reversal , 2009, Vision Research.
[334] D. Melcher. Spatiotopic Transfer of Visual-Form Adaptation across Saccadic Eye Movements , 2005, Current Biology.
[335] Tom C A Freeman,et al. Do we have direct access to retinal image motion during smooth pursuit eye movements? , 2009, Journal of vision.
[336] Erich W. Graf,et al. Extrinsic factors in the perception of bistable motion stimuli , 2010, Vision Research.
[337] N. F. Troje,et al. 2.13 – Biological Motion Perception , 2008 .
[338] David Melcher,et al. Dynamic, object-based remapping of visual features in trans-saccadic perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[339] Heiko Neumann,et al. Interactions of motion and form in visual cortex – A neural model , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[340] Bart Farell,et al. Motion in depth from interocular velocity differences revealed by differential motion aftereffect , 2006, Vision Research.
[341] David Whitney,et al. Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[342] Heather Carnahan,et al. Action–perception dissociation in response to target acceleration , 2002, Vision Research.
[343] J. Enns. Visual binding in the standing wave illusion , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[344] P. Mamassian,et al. Multisensory processing in review: from physiology to behaviour. , 2010, Seeing and perceiving.
[345] Gene R Stoner,et al. Auditory modulation of visual apparent motion with short spatial and temporal intervals. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[346] Davis M. Glasser,et al. Low-level mechanisms do not explain paradoxical motion percepts. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[347] T. Albright,et al. Motion coherency rules are form-cue invariant , 1992, Vision Research.
[348] Alexander Thiele,et al. Speed skills: measuring the visual speed analyzing properties of primate MT neurons , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[349] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Perceptual grouping in the Ternus display: evidence for an `association field' in apparent motion , 2002, Vision Research.
[350] R J Snowden,et al. Stereoscopic Depth Cues Can Segment Motion Information , 1999, Perception.
[351] Monica Gori,et al. Anti-Glass patterns and real motion perception: same or different mechanisms? , 2008, Journal of vision.
[352] Peter Thompson,et al. Velocity after-effects: The effects of adaptation to moving stimuli on the perception of subsequently seen moving stimuli , 1981, Vision Research.
[353] Kiyoshi Fujimoto,et al. Backscroll illusion: Apparent motion in the background of locomotive objects , 2006, Vision Research.
[354] David Whitney,et al. Visually guided reaching depends on motion area MT+. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[355] S. McKee,et al. The precision of binocular and monocular depth judgments in natural settings. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[356] A. T. Smith,et al. Sharpening of drifting, blurred images , 1995, Vision Research.
[357] Alain Berthoz,et al. Estimating the time-to-passage of visual self-motion: Is the second order motion information processed? , 2010, Vision Research.
[358] F. A. Miles,et al. Spatial summation properties of the human ocular following response (OFR): Evidence for nonlinearities due to local and global inhibitory interactions , 2008, Vision Research.
[359] Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al. Spatial distortions and processing latencies in the onset repulsion and Fröhlich effects , 2004, Vision Research.
[360] Philippe Lefèvre,et al. Biological motion drives perception and action. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[361] William Curran,et al. Test stimulus characteristics determine the perceived speed of the dynamic motion aftereffect , 2006, Vision Research.
[362] R. L. Valois,et al. Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors , 1991, Vision Research.
[363] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Independent Aftereffects of Attention and Motion , 2000, Neuron.
[364] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern , 1999, Nature.
[365] Chris J. Tinsley,et al. Processing of first-order motion in marmoset visual cortex is influenced by second-order motion , 2006, Visual Neuroscience.
[366] D J Heeger,et al. Model for the extraction of image flow. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[367] Rufin VanRullen,et al. The Continuous Wagon Wheel Illusion and the ‘When’ Pathway of the Right Parietal Lobe: A Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2008, PloS one.
[368] A. Cobo-Lewis,et al. Dichoptic plaids may rival, but their motions can integrate. , 2000, Spatial vision.
[369] Harold E Bedell,et al. Suppression of motion-produced smear during smooth pursuit eye movements , 1996, Current Biology.
[370] P. Mamassian,et al. The effects of task and saliency on latencies for colour and motion processing , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[371] M. J. Van der Smagt,et al. Context and the Motion Aftereffect: Occlusion Cues in the Test Pattern Alter Perceived Direction , 2002, Perception.
[372] Timothy Ledgeway,et al. Visual adaptation reveals asymmetric spatial frequency tuning for motion. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[373] S. Nishida,et al. Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.
[374] Bart Farell,et al. Shape constancy and depth-order violations in structure from motion: a look at non-frontoparallel axes of rotation. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[375] Dirk Kerzel,et al. Contributions of visible persistence and perceptual set to the flash-lag effect: Focusing on flash onset abolishes the illusion , 2009, Vision Research.
[376] Naoyuki Osaka,et al. Motion aftereffect with flickering test stimuli depends on adapting velocity , 1995, Vision Research.
[377] Kiyoshi Fujimoto,et al. Motion Induction from Biological Motion , 2003, Perception.
[378] J A Beintema,et al. Perception of biological motion without local image motion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[379] J. Hennig,et al. The Processing of First- and Second-Order Motion in Human Visual Cortex Assessed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[380] George Sperling,et al. Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.
[381] Paul A. Warren,et al. A Bayesian Model of Perceived Head-Centered Velocity during Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement , 2010, Current Biology.
[382] Stuart Anstis,et al. Factors affecting footsteps: contrast can change the apparent speed, amplitude and direction of motion , 2004, Vision Research.
[383] M. Lappe,et al. Interaction of stereo vision and optic flow processing revealed by an illusory stimulus , 1998, Vision Research.
[384] Rémy Allard,et al. Exploring the spatiotemporal properties of fractal rotation perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[385] David Whitney,et al. The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object , 2003, Nature.
[386] P. Cavanagh,et al. Bi-directional illusory position shifts toward the end point of apparent motion , 2006, Vision Research.
[387] I. Murakami,et al. A flash-lag effect in random motion , 2001, Vision Research.
[388] C. Duffy,et al. Optic flow illusion and single neuron behaviour reconciled by a population model , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.
[389] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Attentive tracking shifts the perceived location of a nearby flash , 2005, Vision Research.
[390] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.
[391] R. Goutcher,et al. Motion transparency from opposing luminance modulated and contrast modulated gratings , 2009, Vision Research.
[392] M. Morgan,et al. Predicting the motion after-effect from sensitivity loss , 2006, Vision Research.
[393] K. Nakayama,et al. The aperture problem—II. Spatial integration of velocity information along contours , 1988, Vision Research.
[394] N. Troje. Decomposing biological motion: a framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns. , 2002, Journal of vision.
[395] Uwe J. Ilg,et al. The role of areas MT and MST in coding of visual motion underlying the execution of smooth pursuit , 2008, Vision Research.
[396] David Whitney,et al. Second-order motion without awareness: Passive adaptation to second-order motion produces a motion aftereffect , 2007, Vision Research.
[397] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[398] James T. Enns,et al. The path of least persistence: Object status mediates visual updating , 2007, Vision Research.
[399] Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al. Perception of acceleration with short presentation times: Can acceleration be used in interception? , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[400] J. Pettigrew,et al. Does Interhemispheric Competition Mediate Motion-Induced Blindness? A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2003, Perception.
[401] Ayse Pinar Saygin,et al. In the Footsteps of Biological Motion and Multisensory Perception , 2008, Psychological science.
[402] M. Graziano,et al. Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[403] Andreas Bartels,et al. The Coding of Color, Motion, and Their Conjunction in the Human Visual Cortex , 2009, Current Biology.
[404] G. Campana,et al. Separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first- and second-order patterns revealed by rapid forms of visual motion priming and motion aftereffect. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[405] O. Braddick. A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.
[406] Nestor Matthews,et al. Task-specific perceptual learning on speed and direction discrimination , 2003, Vision Research.
[407] V. Lollo,et al. Beyond Visible Persistence: An Alternative Account of Temporal Integration and Segregation in Visual Processing , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.
[408] Tsunehiro Takeda,et al. Alpha band amplification during illusory jitter perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[409] Junghyun Park,et al. Non-veridical visual motion perception immediately after saccades , 2001, Vision Research.
[410] David R Badcock,et al. Independent speed-tuned global-motion systems , 1998, Vision Research.
[411] D. Bradley,et al. Structure and function of visual area MT. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.
[412] P. Cavanagh,et al. Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli , 2000, Vision Research.
[413] Mark Nawrot,et al. Depth from motion parallax scales with eye movement gain. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[414] Monica Gori,et al. Powerful motion illusion caused by temporal asymmetries in ON and OFF visual pathways. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[415] Aaron R. Seitz,et al. What a difference a parameter makes: A psychophysical comparison of random dot motion algorithms , 2009, Vision Research.
[416] Y. Frégnac,et al. Orientation dependent modulation of apparent speed: a model based on the dynamics of feed-forward and horizontal connectivity in V1 cortex , 2002, Vision Research.
[417] David Whitney,et al. Independent coding of object motion and position revealed by distinct contingent aftereffects , 2007, Vision Research.
[418] Scott N.J. Watamaniuk,et al. Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 2003, Vision Research.
[419] T. Ledgeway,et al. Sensitivity to spatial and temporal modulations of first-order and second-order motion , 2006, Vision Research.
[420] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Effects of a static textured background on motion integration , 1995, Vision Research.
[421] A. Watson,et al. Motion-contrast sensitivity: visibility of motion gradients of various spatial frequencies , 1994 .
[422] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Perceived timing of new objects and feature changes. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[423] David Burr,et al. Vision: The World through Picket Fences , 2004, Current Biology.
[424] Edward H Adelson,et al. The geometry of the occluding contour and its effect on motion interpretation. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[425] Yoshimichi Ejima,et al. Effects of luminance contrast and phase difference on motion assimilation for sinusoidal gratings , 1995, Vision Research.
[426] G. Boynton,et al. Effects of feature-based attention on the motion aftereffect at remote locations , 2006, Vision Research.
[427] Marina Pavlova,et al. Prior Knowledge about Display Inversion in Biological Motion Perception , 2003, Perception.
[428] Arash Afraz,et al. Topography of the motion aftereffect with and without eye movements. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[429] K. Nakayama,et al. Adaptation aftereffects in the perception of gender from biological motion. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[430] Gillian S. Barbieri-Hesse,et al. Motion, Flash, and Flicker: A Unified Spatiotemporal Model of Perceived Edge Sharpening , 2003, Perception.
[431] M. Georgeson,et al. Motion contrast: a new metric for direction discrimination , 1997, Vision Research.
[432] P. Cavanagh,et al. Motion adaptation shifts apparent position without the motion aftereffect , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[433] Benjamin T Backus,et al. Illusory motion from change over time in the response to contrast and luminance. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[434] F. Miles,et al. Short-latency ocular following in humans is dependent on absolute (rather than relative) binocular disparity , 2003, Vision Research.
[435] Bevil R. Conway,et al. Neural Basis for a Powerful Static Motion Illusion , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[436] T. Papathomas,et al. Audiovisual short-term influences and aftereffects in motion: examination across three sets of directional pairings. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[437] P. Cavanagh,et al. Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[438] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Space and Time in Perception and Action: The time marker account of cross-channel temporal judgments , 2010 .
[439] A. Georgopoulos,et al. Neurophysiology of perceptual and motor aspects of interception. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[440] Ryota Kanai,et al. Blindness to inconsistent local signals in motion transparency from oscillating dots , 2004, Vision Research.
[441] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Perception of animacy and direction from local biological motion signals. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[442] James M. G. Tsui,et al. Brief motion stimuli preferentially activate surround-suppressed neurons in macaque visual area MT , 2008, Current Biology.
[443] C P Benton,et al. A new approach to analysing texture-defined motion , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[444] Ryota Kanai,et al. Perceptual Alternation Induced by Visual Transients , 2005, Perception.
[445] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. The reference frame of the motion aftereffect is retinotopic. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[446] A. M. Derrington,et al. Errors in direction-of-motion discrimination with dichoptically viewed stimuli , 1993, Vision Research.
[447] Richard J A van Wezel,et al. Velocity Dependence of the Interocular Transfer of Dynamic Motion Aftereffects , 2003, Perception.
[448] Barbara Anne Dosher,et al. Attention mechanisms for multi-location first- and second-order motion perception , 2000, Vision Research.
[449] D. Burr,et al. Motion vision: Are ‘speed lines’ used in human visual motion? , 2000, Current Biology.
[450] I. Murakami,et al. The flash-lag effect as a spatiotemporal correlation structure. , 2001, Journal of vision.
[451] Ikuya Murakami,et al. Illusory jitter in a static stimulus surrounded by a synchronously flickering pattern , 2003, Vision Research.
[452] O J Braddick,et al. Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[453] Takashi R Sato,et al. Perceived Shifts of Flashed Stimuli by Visible and Invisible Object Motion , 2003, Perception.
[454] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[455] R. Blake,et al. Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[456] D. Burr,et al. Contrast sensitivity at high velocities , 1982, Vision Research.
[457] Adriane E Seiffert,et al. Functional MRI studies of human visual motion perception: texture, luminance, attention and after-effects. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[458] S. Dakin,et al. Spatial interference among moving targets , 2005, Vision Research.
[459] Robert Patterson. Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update: comment. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[460] Randolph Blake,et al. Broad tuning for spatial frequency of neural mechanisms underlying visual perception of coherent motion , 1994, Nature.
[461] P. Thompson,et al. Human speed perception is contrast dependent , 1992, Vision Research.
[462] Andrew Glennerster,et al. Stereo and motion parallax cues in human 3D vision: can they vanish without a trace? , 2006, Journal of vision.
[463] Derek H. Arnold,et al. Motion and position coding , 2007, Vision Research.
[464] Tatsuto Takeuchi,et al. PII: S0042-6989(98)00019-4 , 1998 .
[465] Jocelyn Faubert,et al. How is complex second-order motion processed? , 2003, Vision Research.
[466] Hiroshi Ashida,et al. A hierarchical structure of motion system revealed by interocular transfer of flicker motion aftereffects , 2000, Vision Research.
[467] Ning Qian,et al. Effects of attention on motion repulsion , 2005, Vision Research.
[468] D. Burr,et al. Cardinal directions for visual optic flow , 1999, Current Biology.
[469] Stephane J. M. Rainville,et al. The spatial properties of opponent-motion normalization , 2002, Vision Research.
[470] Duje Tadin,et al. Optimal size for perceiving motion decreases with contrast , 2005, Vision Research.
[471] Simon J. Cropper. The detection of motion in chromatic stimuli: Pedestals and masks , 2006, Vision Research.
[472] Szonya Durant,et al. Temporal dependence of local motion induced shifts in perceived position , 2004, Vision Research.
[473] P Cavanagh,et al. Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.
[474] S. Zeki,et al. A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[475] Christopher Patrick Taylor,et al. Aging Reduces Center-Surround Antagonism in Visual Motion Processing , 2005, Neuron.
[476] J M Findlay,et al. Aperture Viewing*: A Review and a Synthesis , 1982, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[477] S. Nishida,et al. Time perception: Brain time or event time? , 2001, Current Biology.
[478] Preeti Verghese,et al. Perceived visual speed constrained by image segmentation , 1996, Nature.
[479] Ian M Thornton,et al. The onset repulsion effect. , 2002, Spatial vision.
[480] Rémy Allard,et al. First- and second-order motion mechanisms are distinct at low but common at high temporal frequencies. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[481] George Mather,et al. A motion-energy model predicts the direction discrimination and MAE duration of two-stroke apparent motion at high and low retinal illuminance , 2010, Vision Research.
[482] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Veridical perception of global motion from disparate component motions , 1998, Vision Research.
[483] John A. Greenwood,et al. The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit , 2005, Vision Research.
[484] Cord Westhoff,et al. Kinematic cues for person identification from biological motion , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[485] Katsumi Watanabe,et al. Object-based anisotropic mislocalization by retinotopic motion signals , 2007, Vision Research.
[486] J. Fodor. The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .
[487] P. Mazzoni,et al. Cross-fixation transfer of motion aftereffects with expansion motion , 2006, Vision Research.
[488] Wang Li,et al. Distortion in perceived image size accompanies flash lag in depth. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[489] E. Castet,et al. Temporal dynamics of motion integration for the initiation of tracking eye movements at ultra-short latencies , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.
[490] Margaret S Livingstone,et al. End-Stopping and the Aperture Problem Two-Dimensional Motion Signals in Macaque V1 , 2003, Neuron.
[491] Edward H. Adelson,et al. Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[492] M. Giese,et al. Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception , 2006, Current Biology.
[493] Martina Poletti,et al. Miniature eye movements enhance fine spatial detail , 2007, Nature.
[494] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Dual multiple-scale processing for motion in the human visual System , 1997, Vision Research.
[495] Markus Lappe,et al. Discrimination of travel distances from ‘situated’ optic flow , 2003, Vision Research.
[496] David Whitney,et al. Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex , 2003, Science.
[497] D. Hubel,et al. The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[498] Elan Barenholtz. Convexities move because they contain matter. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[499] P. Cavanagh,et al. Position-based motion perception for color and texture stimuli: effects of contrast and speed , 1999, Vision Research.
[500] Steven Yantis,et al. Visual interactions in the path of apparent motion , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[501] Jungah Lee,et al. Changes in visual motion perception before saccadic eye movements , 2005, Vision Research.
[502] Vincent Hayward,et al. Motion Aftereffects Transfer between Touch and Vision , 2009, Current Biology.
[503] V. Ramachandran,et al. Illusory Displacement of Equiluminous Kinetic Edges , 1990, Perception.
[504] David Melcher,et al. Spatiotopic temporal integration of visual motion across saccadic eye movements , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[505] Harold E. Bedell,et al. The perception of motion smear during eye and head movements , 2010, Vision Research.
[506] S. Nishida,et al. Positive motion after-effect induced by bandpass-filtered random-dot kinematograms , 1992, Vision Research.
[507] F. Pollick,et al. Expertise with multisensory events eliminates the effect of biological motion rotation on audiovisual synchrony perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[508] David R. Badcock,et al. Motion distorts perceived depth , 2003, Vision Research.
[509] Derek H. Arnold,et al. Motion-induced blindness is not tuned to retinal speed. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[510] T. Womelsdorf,et al. Feature-based attention influences contextual interactions during motion repulsion , 2006, Vision Research.
[511] T. Albright. Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.
[512] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attention-based visual routines: sprites , 2001, Cognition.
[513] George Sperling,et al. Long-lasting sensitization to a given colour after visual search , 2004, Nature.
[514] Luis A. Lesmes,et al. Perceptual motion standstill in rapidly moving chromatic displays. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[515] D. Burr,et al. The “Flash-Lag” Effect Occurs in Audition and Cross-Modally , 2003, Current Biology.
[516] F. A. Miles,et al. The initial ocular following responses elicited by apparent-motion stimuli: Reversal by inter-stimulus intervals , 2006, Vision Research.
[517] T. Albright,et al. Image Segmentation Cues in Motion Processing: Implications for Modularity in Vision , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[518] John A. Greenwood,et al. An extension of the transparent-motion detection limit using speed-tuned global-motion systems , 2006, Vision Research.
[519] R. Sekuler,et al. The independence of channels in human vision selective for direction of movement. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.
[520] T. L. Lewis,et al. Longer VEP latencies and slower reaction times to the onset of second-order motion than to the onset of first-order motion , 2003, Vision Research.
[521] N. Qian,et al. A Physiological Model for Motion-Stereo Integration and a Unified Explanation of Pulfrich-like Phenomena , 1997, Vision Research.
[522] A. Rider,et al. Motion-induced position shifts in global dynamic Gabor arrays. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[523] David C. Burr,et al. The motion aftereffect of transparent motion: Two temporal channels account for perceived direction , 2005, Vision Research.
[524] Markus Lappe,et al. Category-specific interference of object recognition with biological motion perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[525] Katsumi Watanabe,et al. Asymmetric Mislocalization of a Visual Flash Ahead of and behind a Moving Object , 2005, Perception.
[526] D. Alais,et al. Orientation tuning of contrast masking caused by motion streaks. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[527] M. Hawken,et al. Perceived velocity of luminance, chromatic and non-fourier stimuli: Influence of contrast and temporal frequency , 1996, Vision Research.
[528] Takeo Watanabe,et al. Separate Processing of Different Global-Motion Structures in Visual Cortex Is Revealed by fMRI , 2005, Current Biology.
[529] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Shifts in perceived position of flashed stimuli by illusory object motion , 2002, Vision Research.
[530] Tom C.A. Freeman,et al. Simultaneous adaptation of retinal and extra-retinal motion signals , 2007, Vision Research.
[531] Christopher C. Pack,et al. Cortical Mechanisms for the Integration of Visual Motion , 2008 .
[532] David Marr,et al. VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .
[533] B. Mansouri,et al. Binocular influences on global motion processing in the human visual system , 2007, Vision Research.
[534] S. Shimojo,et al. Assimilation-type and Contrast-type Bias of Motion Induced by the Surround in a Random-dot Display: Evidence for Center-Surround Antagonism , 1996, Vision Research.
[535] Wonyeong Sohn,et al. Asymmetric interaction between motion and stereopsis revealed by concurrent adaptation. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[536] F. A. Miles,et al. Initial ocular following in humans: A response to first-order motion energy , 2005, Vision Research.
[537] S. Shimojo,et al. Motion capture changes to induced motion at higher luminance contrasts, smaller eccentricities, and larger inducer sizes , 1993, Vision Research.
[538] Ikuya Murakami,et al. The effects of eccentricity and retinal illuminance on the illusory motion seen in a stationary luminance gradient , 2008, Vision Research.
[539] Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al. Integration of monocular motion signals and the analysis of interocular velocity differences for the perception of motion-in-depth. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[540] Q. Vuong,et al. Incidental Processing of Biological Motion , 2004, Current Biology.
[541] H R Wilson,et al. A model for motion coherence and transparency , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.
[542] Mitsuhiko Hanada. Computational analyses for illusory transformations in the optic flow field and heading perception in the presence of moving objects , 2005, Vision Research.
[543] David Whitaker,et al. Non-veridical size perception of expanding and contracting objects , 1999, Vision Research.
[544] David R. Badcock,et al. Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.
[545] William Curran,et al. Speed tuning of direction repulsion describes an inverted U-function , 2003, Vision Research.
[546] S. Zeki,et al. Temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive systems in the Mondrian world , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[547] Erich W Graf,et al. Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion , 2002, Vision Research.
[548] D. Purves,et al. The wagon-wheel illusion in continuous light , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[549] Zijiang J. He,et al. Surface completion affected by luminance contrast polarity and common motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[550] T. Meese,et al. Spiral mechanisms are required to account for summation of complex motion components , 2002, Vision Research.
[551] P. McOwan,et al. A computational model of the analysis of some first-order and second-order motion patterns by simple and complex cells , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[552] P. McGraw,et al. The segregation and integration of colour in motion processing revealed by motion after-effects , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[553] Maggie Shiffrar,et al. The influence of terminators on motion integration across space , 1992, Vision Research.
[554] D. Burr,et al. Motion psychophysics: 1985–2010 , 2011, Vision Research.
[555] A. Treisman. The binding problem , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[556] M. Lankheet,et al. A gain-control model relating nulling results to the duration of dynamic motion aftereffects , 2003, Vision Research.
[557] Duje Tadin,et al. Visual coherence of moving and stationary image changes , 2002, Vision Research.
[558] Saumil S. Patel,et al. Color and motion: which is the tortoise and which is the hare? , 2003, Vision Research.
[559] Casper J. Erkelens,et al. A single motion system suffices for global-motion perception , 2006, Vision Research.
[560] J. J. Koenderink,et al. Spatial properties of the visual detectability of moving spatial white noise , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[561] Steven C. Dakin,et al. Local and global limitations on direction integration assessed using equivalent noise analysis , 2005, Vision Research.
[562] Terrence J Sejnowski,et al. Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[563] D. Burr,et al. Large receptive fields for optic flow detection in humans , 1998, Vision Research.
[564] W. Newsome,et al. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[565] David Whitney,et al. Second-order motion shifts perceived position , 2006, Vision Research.
[566] S. Gilson,et al. Cue combination for 3D location judgements. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[567] P. Cavanagh,et al. ISI produces reverse apparent motion , 1990, Vision Research.
[568] G. Sperling,et al. The functional architecture of human visual motion perception , 1995, Vision Research.
[569] Taylor R. Hayes,et al. Asymmetric transfer of perceptual learning of luminance- and contrast-modulated motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[570] Amelia R Hunt,et al. Disorganizing biological motion. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[571] James T. Todd,et al. The perception of globally coherent motion , 1992, Vision Research.
[572] N. Qian,et al. Axis-of-motion affects direction discrimination, not speed discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.
[573] A. Metha,et al. Enhanced motion aftereffect for complex motions , 1999, Vision Research.
[574] K. Nakayama,et al. Visual thresholds for shearing motion in monkey and man , 1985, Vision Research.
[575] Erik Blaser,et al. Maximal motion aftereffects in spite of diverted awareness , 2009, Vision Research.
[576] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Gaze modulation of visual aftereffects , 2003, Vision Research.
[577] Harriet A. Allen,et al. Attentional modulation of threshold sensitivity to first-order motion and second-order motion patterns , 2003, Vision Research.
[578] Simon K Rushton,et al. Perception of object trajectory: parsing retinal motion into self and object movement components. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[579] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Spatiotemporal Tuning of Rapid Interactions between Visual-Motion Analysis and Reaching Movement , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[580] H. Wilson,et al. A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[581] George Mather,et al. Two-stroke: A new illusion of visual motion based on the time course of neural responses in the human visual system , 2006, Vision Research.
[582] B. Julesz. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .
[583] A. Johnston,et al. Perceived motion of contrast-modulated gratings: Predictions of the multi-channel gradient model and the role of full-wave rectification , 1995, Vision Research.
[584] David Melcher,et al. The role of attention in central and peripheral motion integration , 2004, Vision Research.
[585] K. D. De Valois,et al. Light adaptation in motion direction judgments. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[586] K. D. Valois,et al. Motion-reversal Reveals Two Motion Mechanisms Functioning in Scotopic Vision , 1997, Vision Research.
[587] Preeti Verghese,et al. Motion grouping impairs speed discrimination , 2006, Vision Research.
[588] Gideon P Caplovitz,et al. The whole moves less than the spin of its parts , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[589] Pete R. Jones,et al. Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation , 2008, Current Biology.
[590] George Mather,et al. Motion-induced position shifts occur after motion integration , 2009, Vision Research.
[591] Hirohisa Yaguchi,et al. Detection of relative and uniform motion. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[592] Stephen T. Hammett. PII: S0042-6989(97)00059-X , 2002 .
[593] J. Schall,et al. Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.
[594] Johannes M. Zanker. PII: S0042-6989(98)00234-X , 1998 .
[595] N Osaka,et al. Inefficient visual search for second-order motion. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[596] Claire V. Hutchinson,et al. Choice reaction times for identifying the direction of first-order motion and different varieties of second-order motion , 2008, Vision Research.
[597] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Center-surround inhibition and facilitation as a function of size and contrast at multiple levels of visual motion processing. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[598] Richard J A van Wezel,et al. The role of motion capture in an illusory transformation of optic flow fields. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[599] Z L Lu,et al. Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[600] D. Badcock,et al. Global motion perception: No interaction between the first- and second-order motion pathways , 1995, Vision Research.
[601] Patrick Cavanagh. Is there low-level motion processing for non-luminance-based stimuli? , 1995 .
[602] David Whitney,et al. Voluntary attention modulates motion-induced mislocalization. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[603] S. Wuerger,et al. The perception of motion in chromatic stimuli. , 2005, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.
[604] S. Anstis,et al. Phi movement as a subtraction process. , 1970, Vision research.
[605] Vic Braden,et al. Recognising the Style of Spatially Exaggerated Tennis Serves , 2001, Perception.
[606] Angelika Lingnau,et al. Selective visual responses to expansion and rotation in the human MT complex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging adaptation , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[607] E. Adelson,et al. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.
[608] Robert J. Snowden,et al. The processing of temporal modulation at different levels of retinal illuminance , 1995, Vision Research.
[609] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Superposition catastrophe and form-motion binding. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[610] Thomas D. Albright,et al. Neural correlates of perceptual motion coherence , 1992, Nature.
[611] H. Ashida,et al. FMRI adaptation reveals separate mechanisms for first-order and second-order motion. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[612] D. Burr,et al. Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.
[613] Sieu K. Khuu,et al. Apparent position in depth of stationary moving three-dimensional objects , 2007, Vision Research.
[614] Alexander Grunewald,et al. Motion repulsion is monocular , 2004, Vision Research.
[615] P. Wenderoth,et al. Retinotopic encoding of the direction aftereffect , 2008, Vision Research.
[616] A. Schofield,et al. Asymmetric transfer of the dynamic motion aftereffect between first- and second-order cues and among different second-order cues. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[617] Thomas U. Otto,et al. The flight path of the phoenix--the visible trace of invisible elements in human vision. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[618] G. Sperling,et al. Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[619] Masashi Tanigawa,et al. Motion assimilation for expansion/contraction and rotation and its spatial properties , 1998, Vision Research.
[620] Stuart Anstis,et al. 'Zigzag motion' goes in unexpected directions. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[621] Bruce C Hansen,et al. Peripheral vision: good for biological motion, bad for signal noise segregation? , 2007, Journal of vision.
[622] D. Burr,et al. Spatial and temporal selectivity of the human motion detection system , 1985, Vision Research.
[623] Branka Spehar,et al. Motion transparency promotes synchronous perceptual binding , 2004, Vision Research.
[624] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. The motion-induced position shift depends on the perceived direction of bistable quartet motion , 2004, Vision Research.
[625] William Curran,et al. The direction aftereffect is driven by adaptation of local motion detectors , 2006, Vision Research.
[626] Akihiro Yagi,et al. Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements Improve Temporal Resolution for Color Perception , 2010, PloS one.
[627] A. Holcombe. Seeing slow and seeing fast: two limits on perception , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[628] Randolph Blake,et al. Fine Temporal Properties of Center–Surround Interactions in Motion Revealed by Reverse Correlation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[629] F. A. Miles,et al. Human ocular following initiated by competing image motions: Evidence for a winner-take-all mechanism , 2006, Vision Research.
[630] David Alais,et al. Motion streaks in fast motion rivalry cause orientation-selective suppression. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[631] R. Nijhawan,et al. Neural delays, visual motion and the flash-lag effect , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[632] E H Adelson,et al. Beyond Junctions: Nonlocal form Constraints on Motion Interpretation , 2001, Perception.
[633] Randolph Blake,et al. Perceptual consequences of centre–surround antagonism in visual motion processing , 2003, Nature.
[634] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Human Brain Activity during Illusory Visual Jitter as Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, Neuron.
[635] Lera Boroditsky,et al. Visual motion aftereffect from understanding motion language , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[636] Kazumichi Matsumiya,et al. Motion mechanisms with different spatiotemporal characteristics identified by an MAE technique with superimposed gratings. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[637] C. Clifford,et al. Illusory motion reversal in tune with motion detectors , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[638] Ignacio Serrano-Pedraza,et al. Evidence for reciprocal antagonism between motion sensors tuned to coarse and fine features. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[639] Ikuya Murakami,et al. Illusory position shift induced by plaid motion , 2009, Vision Research.
[640] Rufin VanRullen,et al. The continuous Wagon Wheel Illusion is object-based , 2006, Vision Research.
[641] J. van Santen,et al. Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[642] Robert J. Snowden,et al. Colour and polarity contributions to global motion perception , 1999, Vision Research.
[643] Z W Pylyshyn,et al. Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.
[644] Zheng Tang,et al. 3D flash lag illusion , 2004, Vision Research.
[645] Timothy Ledgeway,et al. The properties of the motion-detecting mechanisms mediating perceived direction in stochastic displays , 2000, Vision Research.
[646] F. A. Miles,et al. The initial torsional Ocular Following Response (tOFR) in humans: a response to the total motion energy in the stimulus? , 2009, Journal of vision.
[647] Mark Edwards,et al. Motion streaks improve motion detection , 2007, Vision Research.
[648] John Ross,et al. Visual processing of motion , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.
[649] G. DeAngelis,et al. Multisensory integration: psychophysics, neurophysiology, and computation , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[650] Mazyar Fallah,et al. Adaptation of gender derived from biological motion , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[651] David R. Badcock,et al. No interaction of first- and second-order signals in the extraction of global-motion and optic-flow , 2011, Vision Research.
[652] Y. Dan,et al. Asymmetry in Visual Cortical Circuits Underlying Motion-Induced Perceptual Mislocalization , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[653] J. Robson,et al. Discrimination at threshold: Labelled detectors in human vision , 1981, Vision Research.
[654] Joan López-Moliner,et al. Components of motion perception revealed: two different after-effects from a single moving object , 2004, Vision Research.
[655] Tsunehiro Takeda,et al. Close similarity between spatiotemporal frequency tunings of human cortical responses and involuntary manual following responses to visual motion. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[656] C. Koch,et al. The Continuous Wagon Wheel Illusion Is Associated with Changes in Electroencephalogram Power at ∼13 Hz , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[657] R. Freeman,et al. Pulfrich phenomena are coded effectively by a joint motion-disparity process. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[658] Ikuya Murakami,et al. Visual motion detection sensitivity is enhanced by orthogonal induced motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[659] D. Bradley,et al. Velocity computation in the primate visual system , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[660] R. VanRullen. The continuous Wagon Wheel Illusion depends on, but is not identical to neuronal adaptation , 2007, Vision Research.
[661] S. Nishida,et al. Simultaneous motion contrast across space: Involvement of second-order motion? , 1997, Vision Research.
[662] R. Ratcliff. Modeling response signal and response time data , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.
[663] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Continuous perception of motion and shape across saccadic eye movements. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[664] Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al. Hitting moving targets , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[665] Osamu Watanabe,et al. Perceptual costs for motion transparency evaluated by two performance measures , 2009, Vision Research.
[666] G. Mather,et al. The motion aftereffect reloaded , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[667] Laurence R. Harris,et al. Flash lag in depth , 2006, Vision Research.
[668] Randolph Blake,et al. Adaptive center-surround interactions in human vision revealed during binocular rivalry , 2006, Vision Research.
[669] Nikolaus F Troje,et al. Reference Frames for Orientation Anisotropies in Face Recognition and Biological-Motion Perception , 2003, Perception.
[670] P.-J. Hsieh,et al. The infinite regress illusion reveals faulty integration of local and global motion signals , 2006, Vision Research.
[671] Mazyar Fallah,et al. A Motion-Dependent Distortion of Retinotopy in Area V4 , 2006, Neuron.
[672] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Dependence of plaid motion coherence on component grating directions , 1993, Vision Research.
[673] Michael J. Berry,et al. Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina , 1999, Nature.
[674] Bart Krekelberg,et al. Sound and vision , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[675] T. Freeman,et al. Extra-Retinal Vision: Firing at Will , 2007, Current Biology.
[676] K. Duncker,et al. Über induzierte Bewegung , 1929 .
[677] S Anstis,et al. Footsteps and Inchworms: Illusions Show That Contrast Affects Apparent Speed , 2001, Perception.
[678] S. Nishida,et al. Human Visual System Integrates Color Signals along a Motion Trajectory , 2007, Current Biology.
[679] Sieu K. Khuu,et al. Global speed averaging is tuned for binocular disparity , 2006, Vision Research.
[680] Pascal Mamassian,et al. The efficiency of speed discrimination for coherent and transparent motion , 2003, Vision Research.
[681] Antonino Casile,et al. Critical features for the recognition of biological motion. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[682] Tatsuto Takeuchi,et al. Modulation of perceived contrast by a moving surround , 2000, Vision Research.
[683] I. Murakami,et al. Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[684] Norberto M Grzywacz,et al. Measurement of angular velocity in the perception of rotation , 2002, Vision Research.
[685] D. Eagleman,et al. Evidence against the temporal subsampling account of illusory motion reversal. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[686] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Neural dynamics of motion integration and segmentation within and across apertures , 2001, Vision Research.
[687] Craig Aaen-Stockdale,et al. Biological motion perception is cue-invariant. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[688] David Alais,et al. Large shifts in perceived motion direction reveal multiple global motion solutions , 2006, Vision Research.
[689] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Storage for free: a surprising property of a simple gain-control model of motion aftereffects , 2004, Vision Research.
[690] Paul R. Schrater,et al. Perceiving visual expansion without optic flow , 2001, Nature.
[691] Kenji Kawano,et al. Eye movements in response to dichoptic motion: evidence for a parallel-hierarchical structure of visual motion processing in primates. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[692] M. Tarr,et al. Figure-ground assignment to a translating contour: a preference for advancing vs. receding motion. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[693] P. A. Kolers. Aspects of motion perception , 1972 .
[694] T D Albright,et al. Cortical processing of visual motion. , 1993, Reviews of oculomotor research.
[695] Philippe G. Schyns,et al. Attention enhances feature integration , 2003, Vision Research.
[696] George Mather,et al. Distinct position assignment mechanisms revealed by cross-order motion , 2008, Vision Research.
[697] D. Burr,et al. A cortical area that responds specifically to optic flow, revealed by fMRI , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[698] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Center–surround effects on perceived speed , 2010, Vision Research.
[699] P. Viviani,et al. Perceptual asynchronies for biological and non-biological visual events , 2004, Vision Research.
[700] G. DeAngelis,et al. A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[701] Steven C Dakin,et al. The aperture problem in contoured stimuli. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[702] Kenji Yokoi,et al. Object-Based Anisotropies in the Flash-Lag Effect , 2006, Psychological science.
[703] Norberto M Grzywacz,et al. Local computation of angular velocity in rotational visual motion. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[704] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Motion transparency: making models of motion perception transparent , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[705] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[706] Christopher C. Pack,et al. The role of terminators and occlusion cues in motion integration and segmentation: a neural network model , 1999, Vision Research.
[707] Janette Atkinson,et al. Dorsal Stream Vulnerability: Interaction of Intrinsic Programmes and Acquired Developmental Disorders , 2010 .