Neural activation for conceptual identification of correct versus incorrect tool–object pairs

Appropriate tool-object pairing is a natural part of our lives. When preparing to clean our teeth, we know that a toothbrush is useful, but not a screwdriver. The neural correlates of this pairing process remain unclear. We recorded 64-channel electroencephalography to determine neural correlates of identification of tool-object matches and mismatches. Subjects were shown a target tool (e.g. spoon) later paired with an object that was either a conceptual match (e.g. bowl) or mismatch (e.g. wood). To verify that activity was not related to general concept of match-mismatch, in a second condition subjects saw non-tool environmental items (e.g. bird) later paired with a conceptual match (e.g. nest) or mismatch (e.g. spider web). Analysis was focused on time bins after each picture, using standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA). Tool-object match versus mismatch revealed significant differences in the posterior cingulate, precuneus, left insula and superior temporal gyrus. These patterns were not present for environmental match versus mismatch. This work suggests a specific network in comprehending tool-based pairings, but not extensive to other pairings. The posterior cingulate, precuneus, insula and superior temporal gyrus preferentially differentiates tool-object matching and mismatching, identifying a potential locus related to impairments in comprehending appropriate and inappropriate tool-object relationships that arise after neural injury.

[1]  Jonathan Downar,et al.  Neural correlates of the prolonged salience of painful stimulation , 2003, NeuroImage.

[2]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The different neural correlates of action and functional knowledge in semantic memory: an FMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[4]  S. Frey What Puts the How in Where? Tool Use and the Divided Visual Streams Hypothesis , 2007, Cortex.

[5]  JamesW. Lewis Cortical Networks Related to Human Use of Tools , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[6]  B. Mesquita,et al.  Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .

[7]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[8]  J. Moll,et al.  Activation of left intraparietal sulcus using a fMRI conceptual praxis paradigm. , 1998, Arquivos de neuro-psiquiatria.

[9]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Hierarchical error processing: Different errors, different systems , 2007, Brain Research.

[10]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Predictive information and error processing: the role of medial-frontal cortex during motor control. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[12]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of memory retrieval , 1999, Nature.

[13]  T. A. Stroganova,et al.  The right hemisphere fails to respond to temporal novelty in autism: Evidence from an ERP study , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  Guy Vingerhoets,et al.  Knowing about tools: Neural correlates of tool familiarity and experience , 2008, NeuroImage.

[15]  Hellmuth Obrig,et al.  Individual alpha-frequency correlates with amplitude of visual evoked potential and hemodynamic response , 2008, NeuroImage.

[16]  D. E. Fleming,et al.  Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the visual evoked potential , 2008, Vision Research.

[17]  Noriaki Hattori,et al.  Discrete parieto-frontal functional connectivity related to grasping. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Robert L. Whitwell,et al.  The lateral‐occipital and the inferior‐frontal cortex play different roles during the naming of visually presented objects , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[19]  G. Rees,et al.  Covariation of activity in visual and prefrontal cortex associated with subjective visual perception. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Separable neuronal circuitries for manipulable and non-manipulable objects in working memory. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Inferior Temporal, Prefrontal, and Hippocampal Contributions to Visual Working Memory Maintenance and Associative Memory Retrieval , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  J. Jacobs,et al.  Regional dendritic and spine variation in human cerebral cortex: a quantitative golgi study. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval , 2004, Cognition.

[27]  J. Decety,et al.  Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  James L McGaugh,et al.  The consolidation of object and context recognition memory involve different regions of the temporal lobe. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[30]  Ellen M. Migo,et al.  Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Insular cortex is involved in consolidation of object recognition memory. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[33]  R. Adolphs,et al.  NEURAL CORRELATES OF CONCEPTUAL KNOWLEDGE FOR ACTIONS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[35]  K M Heilman,et al.  Conceptual apraxia from lateralized lesions , 1997, Neurology.

[36]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Movement‐related cortical potentials in primary lateral sclerosis , 2006, Annals of neurology.

[37]  Hyowon Lee,et al.  The morphology of supragranular pyramidal neurons in the human insular cortex: a quantitative Golgi study. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[39]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Sensory-motor and cognitive functions of the human posterior parietal cortex involved in manual actions , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[40]  Alex Martin,et al.  Grounding Object Concepts in Perception and Action: Evidence from FMRI Studies of Tools , 2007, Cortex.

[41]  Qian Luo,et al.  The neural substrate of analogical reasoning: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[42]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Changes of error-related ERPs with age , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  Timothy J. Gawne,et al.  The Visual Evoked Potential Is Independent of Surface Alpha Rhythm Phase the Visual Evoked Potential Is Independent of Surface Alpha Rhythm Phase , 2022 .

[44]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  A. Damasio,et al.  A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  M. Trimble,et al.  The insular Lobe of Reil–its Anatamico-Functional, behavioural and Neuropsychiatric attributes in humans–a review , 2004, The world journal of biological psychiatry : the official journal of the World Federation of Societies of Biological Psychiatry.

[47]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Ideomotor apraxia: A review , 2007, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[48]  Sandro Rubichi,et al.  Are visual stimuli sufficient to evoke motor information? Studies with hand primes , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[49]  Justin S. Feinstein,et al.  Further lesion evidence for the neural basis of conceptual knowledge for persons and other concrete entities. , 2008, Journal of neuropsychology.

[50]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Cortical Representations of Personally Familiar Objects and Places: Functional Organization of the Human Posterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  Walter Schneider,et al.  The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[53]  Tilo Kircher,et al.  Lion – tiger – stripes: Neural correlates of indirect semantic priming across processing modalities , 2009, NeuroImage.

[54]  J. Downar,et al.  The Effect of Task Relevance on the Cortical Response to Changes in Visual and Auditory Stimuli: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[55]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[56]  Ken McRae,et al.  Category - Specific semantic deficits , 2008 .

[57]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  What is the role of motor simulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[58]  George S. Cree,et al.  Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.

[59]  Eun Young Kim,et al.  Error-Related Negativity in a Visual Go/No-Go Task: Children vs. Adults , 2007, Developmental neuropsychology.

[60]  T Nagamine,et al.  Blink artifact elimination in electroencephalographic records based on discrete cosine transform domain modeling. , 2001, Frontiers of medical and biological engineering : the international journal of the Japan Society of Medical Electronics and Biological Engineering.

[61]  Martin P. Paulus,et al.  Superior temporal gyrus and insula provide response and outcome-dependent information during assessment and action selection in a decision-making situation , 2005, NeuroImage.

[62]  B. Efron Nonparametric estimates of standard error: The jackknife, the bootstrap and other methods , 1981 .

[63]  Markus Graf,et al.  The role of action representations in visual object recognition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[64]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[65]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  Alex Martin,et al.  Experience-dependent modulation of category-related cortical activity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.