Addressing the effect of concern with COVID-19 threat on prejudice towards immigrants: The sequential mediating role of need for cognitive closure and desire for cultural tightness

[1]  M. Rubini,et al.  At the roots of attribution of human rights to migrants , 2022, Frontiers in Psychology.

[2]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  By all means necessary: Closed mindedness, ingroup morality and weapon ownership , 2022, European Journal of Social Psychology.

[3]  M. Gelfand,et al.  The Moral Foundations of Desired Cultural Tightness , 2022, Frontiers in Psychology.

[4]  A. Pierro,et al.  Perceived COVID-19 Threat and Reactions to Noncompliant Health-Protective Behaviors: The Mediating Role of Desired Cultural Tightness and the Moderating Role of Age , 2022, International journal of environmental research and public health.

[5]  Carlos A. Almenara,et al.  Concern with COVID-19 pandemic threat and attitudes towards immigrants: The mediating effect of the desire for tightness , 2021, Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology.

[6]  M. Gelfand,et al.  The Mediational Role of Desire for Cultural Tightness on Concern With COVID-19 and Perceived Self-Control , 2021, Frontiers in Psychology.

[7]  E. Crocetti,et al.  Development of prejudice against immigrants and ethnic minorities in adolescence: A systematic review with meta-analysis of longitudinal studies , 2021 .

[8]  A. Pierro,et al.  COVID-19 and prejudice against migrants: the mediating roles of need for cognitive closure and binding moral foundations. A comparative study , 2021, The Journal of social psychology.

[9]  Dana S. Nau,et al.  The relationship between cultural tightness–looseness and COVID-19 cases and deaths: a global analysis , 2021, The Lancet Planetary Health.

[10]  E. Crocetti,et al.  Prejudice and Inclusiveness in Adolescence: The Role of Social Dominance Orientation and Multiple Categorization. , 2020, Child development.

[11]  L. G. Conway,et al.  Social Psychological Measurements of COVID-19: Coronavirus Perceived Threat, Government Response, Impacts, and Experiences Questionnaires , 2020 .

[12]  A. Sorokowska,et al.  Can Information about Pandemics Increase Negative Attitudes toward Foreign Groups? A Case of COVID-19 Outbreak , 2020, Sustainability.

[13]  Nick Chater,et al.  Facing up to the uncertainties of COVID-19 , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.

[14]  M. Hewstone,et al.  “They are all alike”: When negative minority outgroups are generalized onto superordinate inclusive outgroups , 2019, International Journal of Intercultural Relations.

[15]  Vidar Schei,et al.  Ecological and cultural factors underlying the global distribution of prejudice , 2019, PloS one.

[16]  A. Pierro,et al.  Motivated prejudice: The effect of need for closure on anti-immigrant attitudes in the United States and Italy and the mediating role of binding moral foundations , 2019, International Journal of Intercultural Relations.

[17]  Kimberly Rios,et al.  An experimental approach to Intergroup Threat Theory: Manipulations, moderators, and consequences of realistic vs. symbolic threat , 2018 .

[18]  M. Gelfand,et al.  The Strength of Social Norms Across Human Groups , 2017, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[19]  M. Rubini,et al.  Linguistic Discrimination Toward Roma: Can Intergroup Threat Enhance Bias? , 2017 .

[20]  M. Hewstone,et al.  Generalisation of Roma onto Romanians: Evidence of the outgroup projection effect , 2017 .

[21]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Can a Naturally Occurring Pathogen Threat Change Social Attitudes? Evaluations of Gay Men and Lesbians During the 2014 Ebola Epidemic , 2016 .

[22]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Epistemic motivation and perpetuation of group culture: Effects of need for cognitive closure on trans-generational norm transmission , 2015 .

[23]  Dana S. Nau,et al.  Societal threat and cultural variation in the strength of social norms: An evolutionary basis , 2015 .

[24]  M. Rubini,et al.  The Role of Reduced Humanity in Producing Linguistic Discrimination , 2015, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[25]  Mark Rubin,et al.  Negative intergroup contact is more influential, but positive intergroup contact is more common: Assessing contact prominence and contact prevalence in five Central European countries , 2014 .

[26]  Michele J. Gelfand,et al.  Tightness–looseness across the 50 united states , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Alessandro Bergamaschi Adolescents and prejudice: A comparative study of the attitudes of two European adolescent populations regarding the issues that are raised by increasing cultural and religious pluralism☆ , 2013 .

[28]  L. Mannetti,et al.  Ethnic Categorization: The Role of Epistemic Motivation, Prejudice, and Perceived Threat , 2012 .

[29]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Mapping the moral domain. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Kibum Kim,et al.  Differences Between Tight and Loose Cultures: A 33-Nation Study , 2011, Science.

[31]  J. Vala,et al.  From prejudice to discrimination: The legitimizing role of perceived threat in discrimination against immigrants , 2010 .

[32]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  J. Leyens,et al.  From infra-humanization to discrimination: The mediation of symbolic threat needs egalitarian norms , 2009 .

[34]  K Jack Ishak,et al.  Meta-analysis of longitudinal studies , 2007, Clinical trials.

[35]  Craig K. Enders,et al.  Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. , 2007, Psychological methods.

[36]  E. Higgins,et al.  "On the move" or "Staying put": Locomotion, need for closure, and reactions to organizational change. , 2007 .

[37]  José Arturo Saavedra Casco The Language of the Young People , 2006 .

[38]  Linda R. Tropp,et al.  Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes a Meta-analytic Test of Intergroup Contact Theory , 2022 .

[39]  Sooyeon Kim,et al.  The Impact of Misspecified Item Parceling on Representing Latent Variables in Covariance Structure Modeling: A Simulation Study , 2003 .

[40]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  When similarity breeds content: need for closure and the allure of homogeneous and self-resembling groups. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  T. Little,et al.  To Parcel or Not to Parcel: Exploring the Question, Weighing the Merits , 2002 .

[42]  P. Scheepers,et al.  Ethnic Exclusionism in European Countries. Public Opposition to Civil Rights for Legal Migrants as a Response to Perceived Ethnic Threat , 2002 .

[43]  G. Mead,et al.  Essays on Social Psychology , 2001 .

[44]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  Karen Stenner,et al.  Perceived Threat and Authoritarianism , 1997 .

[46]  A. Dijksterhuis,et al.  Motivated Social Cognition: Need for Closure Effects on Memory and Judgment , 1996 .

[47]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated closing of the mind: "seizing" and "freezing". , 1996, Psychological review.

[48]  Sandra L. Wood,et al.  With Malice Toward Some: Refining the Model – The Role of Antecedent Considerations as Individual Differences , 1995 .

[49]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  V. Yzerbyt,et al.  The Ingroup Overexclusion Effect - Impact of Valence and Confirmation On Stereotypical Information Search , 1992 .

[51]  D. Ward Right-Wing Authoritarianism , 1982, American Political Science Review.

[52]  J Tudor-Hart,et al.  On the nature of prejudice. , 1961, The Eugenics review.

[53]  J. Vos The Psychology of Covid-19: Building Resilience for Future Pandemics , 2021 .

[54]  J. Vala,et al.  Immigrants and Refugees: From Social Disaffection to Perceived Threat , 2020 .

[55]  A. Roets,et al.  Advances in Experimental Social Psychology , 2015 .

[56]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  Lay Epistemics and Human Knowledge: Cognitive and Motivational Bases , 2013 .

[57]  Luigia Simona Sica,et al.  Identity styles, dimensions, statuses, and functions: Making connections among identity conceptualizations , 2013 .

[58]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  Danger, Disease, and the Nature of Prejudice(s) , 2012 .

[59]  Flavia Albarello,et al.  Relazioni intergruppi e fenomeni di deumanizzazione. , 2008 .

[60]  Organización de las Naciones Unidas International Migration Report 2002 , 2006 .

[61]  Arie W Kruglanski,et al.  Groups as epistemic providers: need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. , 2006, Psychological review.

[62]  J. Duckitt,et al.  A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice , 2001 .

[63]  J. Dovidio,et al.  The Immigration Dilemma: The Role of Perceived Group Competition, Ethnic Prejudice, and National Identity , 2001 .

[64]  W. Stephan,et al.  An Integrated Threat Theory of Prejudice , 2000 .

[65]  Stuart Oskamp,et al.  Reducing prejudice and discrimination , 2000 .

[66]  F. Pratto,et al.  Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression , 1999 .

[67]  V. Yzerbyt,et al.  The ingroup overexclusion effect: Identity concerns in decisions about group membership , 1995 .

[68]  G. Tapinos International migration and development. , 1994, Population bulletin of the United Nations.

[69]  A. Kruglanski Lay epistemics and human knowledge , 1989 .