Addressing the effect of concern with COVID-19 threat on prejudice towards immigrants: The sequential mediating role of need for cognitive closure and desire for cultural tightness
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Rubini,et al. At the roots of attribution of human rights to migrants , 2022, Frontiers in Psychology.
[2] A. Kruglanski,et al. By all means necessary: Closed mindedness, ingroup morality and weapon ownership , 2022, European Journal of Social Psychology.
[3] M. Gelfand,et al. The Moral Foundations of Desired Cultural Tightness , 2022, Frontiers in Psychology.
[4] A. Pierro,et al. Perceived COVID-19 Threat and Reactions to Noncompliant Health-Protective Behaviors: The Mediating Role of Desired Cultural Tightness and the Moderating Role of Age , 2022, International journal of environmental research and public health.
[5] Carlos A. Almenara,et al. Concern with COVID-19 pandemic threat and attitudes towards immigrants: The mediating effect of the desire for tightness , 2021, Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology.
[6] M. Gelfand,et al. The Mediational Role of Desire for Cultural Tightness on Concern With COVID-19 and Perceived Self-Control , 2021, Frontiers in Psychology.
[7] E. Crocetti,et al. Development of prejudice against immigrants and ethnic minorities in adolescence: A systematic review with meta-analysis of longitudinal studies , 2021 .
[8] A. Pierro,et al. COVID-19 and prejudice against migrants: the mediating roles of need for cognitive closure and binding moral foundations. A comparative study , 2021, The Journal of social psychology.
[9] Dana S. Nau,et al. The relationship between cultural tightness–looseness and COVID-19 cases and deaths: a global analysis , 2021, The Lancet Planetary Health.
[10] E. Crocetti,et al. Prejudice and Inclusiveness in Adolescence: The Role of Social Dominance Orientation and Multiple Categorization. , 2020, Child development.
[11] L. G. Conway,et al. Social Psychological Measurements of COVID-19: Coronavirus Perceived Threat, Government Response, Impacts, and Experiences Questionnaires , 2020 .
[12] A. Sorokowska,et al. Can Information about Pandemics Increase Negative Attitudes toward Foreign Groups? A Case of COVID-19 Outbreak , 2020, Sustainability.
[13] Nick Chater,et al. Facing up to the uncertainties of COVID-19 , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.
[14] M. Hewstone,et al. “They are all alike”: When negative minority outgroups are generalized onto superordinate inclusive outgroups , 2019, International Journal of Intercultural Relations.
[15] Vidar Schei,et al. Ecological and cultural factors underlying the global distribution of prejudice , 2019, PloS one.
[16] A. Pierro,et al. Motivated prejudice: The effect of need for closure on anti-immigrant attitudes in the United States and Italy and the mediating role of binding moral foundations , 2019, International Journal of Intercultural Relations.
[17] Kimberly Rios,et al. An experimental approach to Intergroup Threat Theory: Manipulations, moderators, and consequences of realistic vs. symbolic threat , 2018 .
[18] M. Gelfand,et al. The Strength of Social Norms Across Human Groups , 2017, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[19] M. Rubini,et al. Linguistic Discrimination Toward Roma: Can Intergroup Threat Enhance Bias? , 2017 .
[20] M. Hewstone,et al. Generalisation of Roma onto Romanians: Evidence of the outgroup projection effect , 2017 .
[21] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Can a Naturally Occurring Pathogen Threat Change Social Attitudes? Evaluations of Gay Men and Lesbians During the 2014 Ebola Epidemic , 2016 .
[22] D. A. Kenny,et al. Epistemic motivation and perpetuation of group culture: Effects of need for cognitive closure on trans-generational norm transmission , 2015 .
[23] Dana S. Nau,et al. Societal threat and cultural variation in the strength of social norms: An evolutionary basis , 2015 .
[24] M. Rubini,et al. The Role of Reduced Humanity in Producing Linguistic Discrimination , 2015, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[25] Mark Rubin,et al. Negative intergroup contact is more influential, but positive intergroup contact is more common: Assessing contact prominence and contact prevalence in five Central European countries , 2014 .
[26] Michele J. Gelfand,et al. Tightness–looseness across the 50 united states , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[27] Alessandro Bergamaschi. Adolescents and prejudice: A comparative study of the attitudes of two European adolescent populations regarding the issues that are raised by increasing cultural and religious pluralism☆ , 2013 .
[28] L. Mannetti,et al. Ethnic Categorization: The Role of Epistemic Motivation, Prejudice, and Perceived Threat , 2012 .
[29] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Mapping the moral domain. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[30] Kibum Kim,et al. Differences Between Tight and Loose Cultures: A 33-Nation Study , 2011, Science.
[31] J. Vala,et al. From prejudice to discrimination: The legitimizing role of perceived threat in discrimination against immigrants , 2010 .
[32] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[33] J. Leyens,et al. From infra-humanization to discrimination: The mediation of symbolic threat needs egalitarian norms , 2009 .
[34] K Jack Ishak,et al. Meta-analysis of longitudinal studies , 2007, Clinical trials.
[35] Craig K. Enders,et al. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. , 2007, Psychological methods.
[36] E. Higgins,et al. "On the move" or "Staying put": Locomotion, need for closure, and reactions to organizational change. , 2007 .
[37] José Arturo Saavedra Casco. The Language of the Young People , 2006 .
[38] Linda R. Tropp,et al. Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes a Meta-analytic Test of Intergroup Contact Theory , 2022 .
[39] Sooyeon Kim,et al. The Impact of Misspecified Item Parceling on Representing Latent Variables in Covariance Structure Modeling: A Simulation Study , 2003 .
[40] A. Kruglanski,et al. When similarity breeds content: need for closure and the allure of homogeneous and self-resembling groups. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[41] T. Little,et al. To Parcel or Not to Parcel: Exploring the Question, Weighing the Merits , 2002 .
[42] P. Scheepers,et al. Ethnic Exclusionism in European Countries. Public Opposition to Civil Rights for Legal Migrants as a Response to Perceived Ethnic Threat , 2002 .
[43] G. Mead,et al. Essays on Social Psychology , 2001 .
[44] A. Greenwald,et al. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[45] Karen Stenner,et al. Perceived Threat and Authoritarianism , 1997 .
[46] A. Dijksterhuis,et al. Motivated Social Cognition: Need for Closure Effects on Memory and Judgment , 1996 .
[47] A. Kruglanski,et al. Motivated closing of the mind: "seizing" and "freezing". , 1996, Psychological review.
[48] Sandra L. Wood,et al. With Malice Toward Some: Refining the Model – The Role of Antecedent Considerations as Individual Differences , 1995 .
[49] A. Kruglanski,et al. Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[50] V. Yzerbyt,et al. The Ingroup Overexclusion Effect - Impact of Valence and Confirmation On Stereotypical Information Search , 1992 .
[51] D. Ward. Right-Wing Authoritarianism , 1982, American Political Science Review.
[52] J Tudor-Hart,et al. On the nature of prejudice. , 1961, The Eugenics review.
[53] J. Vos. The Psychology of Covid-19: Building Resilience for Future Pandemics , 2021 .
[54] J. Vala,et al. Immigrants and Refugees: From Social Disaffection to Perceived Threat , 2020 .
[55] A. Roets,et al. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology , 2015 .
[56] Arie W. Kruglanski,et al. Lay Epistemics and Human Knowledge: Cognitive and Motivational Bases , 2013 .
[57] Luigia Simona Sica,et al. Identity styles, dimensions, statuses, and functions: Making connections among identity conceptualizations , 2013 .
[58] Steven L. Neuberg,et al. Danger, Disease, and the Nature of Prejudice(s) , 2012 .
[59] Flavia Albarello,et al. Relazioni intergruppi e fenomeni di deumanizzazione. , 2008 .
[60] Organización de las Naciones Unidas. International Migration Report 2002 , 2006 .
[61] Arie W Kruglanski,et al. Groups as epistemic providers: need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. , 2006, Psychological review.
[62] J. Duckitt,et al. A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice , 2001 .
[63] J. Dovidio,et al. The Immigration Dilemma: The Role of Perceived Group Competition, Ethnic Prejudice, and National Identity , 2001 .
[64] W. Stephan,et al. An Integrated Threat Theory of Prejudice , 2000 .
[65] Stuart Oskamp,et al. Reducing prejudice and discrimination , 2000 .
[66] F. Pratto,et al. Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression , 1999 .
[67] V. Yzerbyt,et al. The ingroup overexclusion effect: Identity concerns in decisions about group membership , 1995 .
[68] G. Tapinos. International migration and development. , 1994, Population bulletin of the United Nations.
[69] A. Kruglanski. Lay epistemics and human knowledge , 1989 .