Collinear facilitation and suppression at the periphery

Collinear facilitation is a common phenomenon in the fovea, but it has been recently challenged at the human periphery. Since physiological studies show that facilitation is found at the periphery but only from outside the receptive field, our hypothesis was that facilitation at the periphery exists but from larger target-flanker separations than the fovea. Here, we applied a recent paradigm (Polat & Sagi, 2007) to probe facilitation at the periphery. We used a Yes/No detection task by measuring the false-positive reports (false-alarm, pfa) and hit-rate (phit) for a low-contrast Gabor target (between two flankers) that appeared randomly at the fovea or at the periphery (2° or 4°) to the right or left side. We used different target-flanker separations and orientations at the fovea and at the periphery. Importantly, we found that phit is affected by the target-flanker separations and orientations. Short distances show a suppression effect, but the range of suppression increases with increasing eccentricity. A facilitation effect was found for collinear configuration outside of the suppression range. A similar effect was found for the decisional criterion (Cr), which was correlated with suppression (positive) and facilitation (negative). All together, our results indicate that facilitation exists at the periphery when the target-flanker distance is properly scaled. Thus, our results indicate that collinear facilitation is a common phenomenon that exists in both the periphery and fovea. The suppression range indicates that the perceptual receptive field increases with increasing eccentricity. Our results provide a working hypothesis that explains the functional differences found between the fovea and the periphery. This supports the basic phenomena underlying visual perception, such as collinear facilitation, visual crowding, and backward masking.

[1]  U. Polat,et al.  The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions , 1994, Vision Research.

[2]  Hamutal Slovin,et al.  Population response to contextual influences in the primary visual cortex. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  J. Movshon,et al.  Selectivity and spatial distribution of signals from the receptive field surround in macaque V1 neurons. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  D. Sagi,et al.  Isolating Excitatory and Inhibitory Nonlinear Spatial Interactions Involved in Contrast Detection * * Part of this paper was presented at the 17th ECVP conference, Eindhoven, The Netherlands (September 1994). , 1996, Vision Research.

[5]  U. Polat,et al.  Contrast response characteristics of long-range lateral interactions in cat striate cortex , 2001, Neuroreport.

[6]  U. Polat Functional architecture of long-range perceptual interactions. , 1999, Spatial vision.

[7]  M. Morgan,et al.  Facilitation from collinear flanks is cancelled by non-collinear flanks , 2000, Vision Research.

[8]  R. Frostig,et al.  Cortical point-spread function and long-range lateral interactions revealed by real-time optical imaging of macaque monkey primary visual cortex , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  U. Polat,et al.  What pattern the eye sees best , 1999, Vision Research.

[10]  S. Klein,et al.  Detection and discrimination of the direction of motion in central and peripheral vision of normal and amblyopic observers , 1984, Vision Research.

[11]  D. Heeger,et al.  Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppression and enhancement , 2001, Vision Research.

[12]  D. Sagi,et al.  Excitatory-inhibitory network in the visual cortex: psychophysical evidence. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys , 1995, Neuron.

[14]  R F Hess,et al.  Relationship between facilitation at threshold and suprathreshold contour integration. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[15]  S Marcelja,et al.  Mathematical description of the responses of simple cortical cells. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  E. Peli,et al.  Lateral interactions: size does matter , 2002, Vision Research.

[17]  U. Polat,et al.  Major Depression Affects Perceptual Filling-In , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[18]  W Singer,et al.  The Perceptual Grouping Criterion of Colinearity is Reflected by Anisotropies of Connections in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  C. Gilbert Horizontal integration and cortical dynamics , 1992, Neuron.

[20]  Bettina L. Beard,et al.  Vernier Acuity with Non-simultaneous Targets: The Cortical Magnification Factor Estimated by Psychophysics , 1997, Vision Research.

[21]  A Gorea,et al.  Failure to handle more than one internal representation in visual detection tasks. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[23]  U. Polat,et al.  Collinear stimuli regulate visual responses depending on cell's contrast threshold , 1998, Nature.

[24]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  Uri Polat,et al.  The relationship between the subjective and objective aspects of visual filling-in , 2007, Vision Research.

[26]  J. Movshon,et al.  Nature and interaction of signals from the receptive field center and surround in macaque V1 neurons. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Disinhibition Outside Receptive Fields in the Visual Cortex , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Relationships between horizontal interactions and functional architecture in cat striate cortex as revealed by cross-correlation analysis , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Functional organization of the visual cortex. , 1983, Progress in brain research.

[30]  C. Koch,et al.  Flanker effects in peripheral contrast discrimination—psychophysics and modeling , 2001, Vision Research.

[31]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Columnar specificity of intrinsic horizontal and corticocortical connections in cat visual cortex , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  David Alais,et al.  The mechanisms of collinear integration. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[33]  D. Sagi,et al.  Effects of spatial configuration on contrast detection , 1998, Vision Research.

[34]  Siegrid Löwel,et al.  GABA-inactivation attenuates colinear facilitation in cat primary visual cortex , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  Mylène C. Q. Farias,et al.  Detection of Gabor patterns of different sizes, shapes, phases and eccentricities , 2007, Vision Research.

[36]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Circuits for Local and Global Signal Integration in Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  H. B. Barlow,et al.  What does the eye see best? , 1983, Nature.

[38]  Charles D. Gilbert,et al.  The Role of Horizontal Connections in Generating Long Receptive Fields in the Cat Visual Cortex , 1989, The European journal of neuroscience.

[39]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  On the perception of illusory contours , 1994, Vision Research.

[40]  U Polat,et al.  Spatial interactions in human vision: from near to far via experience-dependent cascades of connections. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Shifts in perceived size due to masking , 1983, Vision Research.

[42]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[43]  D. Sagi,et al.  Recurrent networks in human visual cortex: psychophysical evidence. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[44]  S. Klein,et al.  Vernier acuity, crowding and cortical magnification , 1985, Vision Research.

[45]  Eli Peli,et al.  Facilitation of contrast detection in near-peripheral vision , 2004, Vision Research.

[46]  Robert O. Duncan,et al.  Cortical Magnification within Human Primary Visual Cortex Correlates with Acuity Thresholds , 2003, Neuron.

[47]  J Sergent,et al.  An investigation into perceptual completion in blind areas of the visual field. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  T. Kasamatsu,et al.  Collinear facilitation is independent of receptive-field expansion at low contrast , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  Cm VERNIER ACUITY AS LINE AND DIPOLE DETECTION * , 2002 .

[50]  U Polat,et al.  Facilitation and suppression of single striate-cell activity by spatially discrete pattern stimuli presented beyond the receptive field , 2001, Visual Neuroscience.

[51]  D. Levi,et al.  Receptive versus perceptive fields from the reverse-correlation viewpoint , 2006, Vision Research.

[52]  C. Gilbert Horizontal integration in the neocortex , 1985, Trends in Neurosciences.

[53]  U. Polat,et al.  Excitatory repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex does not affect perceptual filling-in in healthy volunteers , 2011, Vision Research.

[54]  Chien-Chung Chen,et al.  Distinguishing lateral interaction from uncertainty reduction in collinear flanker effect on contrast discrimination. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[55]  Claude Bonnet,et al.  Psychophysical measures of illusory form perception: Further evidence for local mechanisms , 1993, Vision Research.

[56]  C. Tyler,et al.  Lateral sensitivity modulation explains the flanker effect in contrast discrimination , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[57]  A Grinvald,et al.  Optical imaging reveals the functional architecture of neurons processing shape and motion in owl monkey area MT , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[58]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[59]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Shifts in perceived size as a function of contrast and temporal modulation , 1983, Vision Research.

[60]  Dov Sagi,et al.  Eccentricity effects on lateral interactions , 2005, Vision Research.

[61]  David Whitaker,et al.  Modelling of orientation discrimination across the visual field , 1993, Vision Research.

[62]  U. Polat,et al.  Neurophysiological Evidence for Contrast Dependent Long-range Facilitation and Suppression in the Human Visual Cortex , 1996, Vision Research.

[63]  D. Fitzpatrick The functional organization of local circuits in visual cortex: insights from the study of tree shrew striate cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  D. Whitteridge,et al.  The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[65]  J. Rovamo,et al.  An estimation and application of the human cortical magnification factor , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Clustered intrinsic connections in cat visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[67]  R. Vautin,et al.  Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[68]  S. McKee,et al.  The effect of spatial configuration on surround suppression of contrast sensitivity. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[69]  A. Grinvald,et al.  Relationship between intrinsic connections and functional architecture revealed by optical imaging and in vivo targeted biocytin injections in primate striate cortex. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[70]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Intrinsic connectivity and receptive field properties in visual cortex , 1985, Vision Research.

[71]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[72]  A. Watson Summation of grating patches indicates many types of detector at one retinal location , 1982, Vision Research.