Neural Changes after Operant Conditioning of the Aerial Respiratory Behavior in Lymnaea stagnalis

In this study, we demonstrate neural changes that occurred during operant conditioning of the aerial respiratory behavior ofLymnaea stagnalis. Aerial respiration inLymnaea occurs at the water interface and is achieved by opening and closing movements of its respiratory orifice, the pneumostome. This behavior is controlled by a central pattern generator (CPG), the neurons of which, as well as the motoneurons innervating the pneumostome, have previously been identified and their synaptic connections well characterized. The respiratory behavior was operantly conditioned by applying a mechanical stimulus to the open pneumostome whenever the animal attempted to breathe. This negative reinforcement to the open pneumostome resulted in its immediate closure and a significant reduction in the overall respiratory activity. Electrophysiological recordings from the isolated CNSs after operant conditioning showed that the spontaneous patterned respiratory activity of the CPG neurons was significantly reduced. This included reduced spontaneous activity of the CPG interneuron involved in pneumostome opening (input 3 interneuron) and a reduced frequency of spontaneous tonic activity of the CPG interneuron [right pedal dorsal 1 (RPeD1)]. The ability to trigger the patterned respiratory activity by electrical stimulation of RPeD1 was also significantly reduced after operant conditioning. This study therefore demonstrates significant changes within a CPG that are associated with changes in a rhythmic homeostatic behavior after operant conditioning.

[1]  G. Hoyle Learning, using natural reinforcements, in insect preparations that permit cellular neuronal analysis. , 1980, Journal of neurobiology.

[2]  G. Kemenes,et al.  Training in a novel environment improves the appetitive learning performance of the snail, Lymnaea stagnalis. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[3]  C. H. Bailey,et al.  The anatomy of a memory: convergence of results across a diversity of tests , 1988, Trends in Neurosciences.

[4]  T. Carew,et al.  Invertebrate learning and memory: from behavior to molecules. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  A. Bulloch,et al.  In vitro reconstruction of the respiratory central pattern generator of the mollusk Lymnaea. , 1990, Science.

[6]  K. Lukowiak,et al.  Hypoxia-induced respiratory patterned activity in Lymnaea originates at the periphery. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  N. Mackintosh The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .

[8]  E. Ringseis,et al.  Operant conditioning of aerial respiratory behaviour in Lymnaea stagnalis , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[9]  T. Carew,et al.  Operant conditioning of head-waving in Aplysia. II. Contingent modification of electromyographic activity in identified muscles , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  D. A. Baxter,et al.  Contingent-Dependent Enhancement of Rhythmic Motor Patterns: AnIn Vitro Analog of Operant Conditioning , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  E Feldman,et al.  Learned changes of feeding behavior in Aplysia in response to edible and inedible foods , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Cook,et al.  Operant conditioning of head-waving in Aplysia. III. Cellular analysis of possible reinforcement pathways , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  J R Wolpaw,et al.  Operant conditioning of H-reflex changes synaptic terminals on primate motoneurons. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  T. Carew,et al.  Operant conditioning of head-waving in Aplysia. I. Identified muscles involved in the operant response , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  W. Winlow,et al.  The distribution of three wide-acting synaptic inputs to identified neurons in the isolated brain of , 1981 .

[16]  K. Lukowiak,et al.  Identification of a putative mechanosensory neuron in Lymnaea: characterization of its synaptic and functional connections with the whole-body withdrawal interneuron. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  G. Horridge,et al.  Learning of Leg Position by Headless Insects , 1962, Nature.

[18]  Xiang Yang Chen,et al.  Operant conditioning of H-reflex in freely moving rats. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  K. Lukowiak,et al.  Inhibition of the respiratory pattern-generating neurons by an identified whole-body withdrawal interneuron of Lymnaea stagnalis , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[20]  R. Forman Leg position learning by an insect. I. A heat avoidance learning paradigm. , 1984, Journal of neurobiology.

[21]  E. Ringseis,et al.  Long-term memory of an operantly conditioned respiratory behaviour pattern in lymnaea stagnalis , 1998, The Journal of experimental biology.

[22]  L. Tsitolovsky,et al.  Instrumental conditioning of the activity of putative command neurons in the mollusk Helix , 1997, Brain Research.

[23]  Shin Nagayama,et al.  Enhancement of an inhibitory input to the feeding central pattern generator in Lymnaea stagnalis during conditioned taste-aversion learning , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[24]  G. Hoyle,et al.  Neural events underlying learning in insects: changes in pacemaker , 1977, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[25]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A lower bound on the detectability of nonassociative learning in the local bending reflex of the medicinal leech. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.