Modeling Driver Behavior in a Cognitive Architecture

Objective: This paper explores the development of a rigorous computational model of driver behavior in a cognitive architecture--a computational framework with underlying psychological theories that incorporate basic properties and limitations of the human system. Background: Computational modeling has emerged as a powerful tool for studying the complex task of driving, allowing researchers to simulate driver behavior and explore the parameters and constraints of this behavior. Method: An integrated driver model developed in the ACT-R (Adaptive Control of Thought-Rational) cognitive architecture is described that focuses on the component processes of control, monitoring, and decision making in a multilane highway environment. Results: This model accounts for the steering profiles, lateral position profiles, and gaze distributions of human drivers during lane keeping, curve negotiation, and lane changing. Conclusion: The model demonstrates how cognitive architectures facilitate understanding of driver behavior in the context of general human abilities and constraints and how the driving domain benefits cognitive architectures by pushing model development toward more complex, realistic tasks. Application: The model can also serve as a core computational engine for practical applications that predict and recognize driver behavior and distraction.

[1]  D. M. Hoedemaeker,et al.  Modeling driver behaviour with different degrees of automation : a hierarchical decision framework of interacting mental models , 1998 .

[2]  David H. Weir,et al.  Measurement and Interpretation of Driver/Vehicle System Dynamic Response , 1973 .

[3]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Serial modules in parallel: the psychological refractory period and perfect time-sharing. , 2001, Psychological review.

[4]  Michael Matessa,et al.  A Production System Theory of Serial Memory , 1997 .

[5]  David H. Weir,et al.  New Results in Driver Steering Control Models , 1977 .

[6]  Leonard Evans,et al.  Human behavior and traffic safety , 2011 .

[7]  Hans Godthelp,et al.  Speed Choice and Steering Behavior in Curve Driving , 1996, Hum. Factors.

[8]  Erwin R. Boer,et al.  Toward an Integrated Model of Driver Behavior in Cognitive Architecture , 2001 .

[9]  Wai-Tat Fu,et al.  Soft constraints in interactive behavior: the case of ignoring perfect knowledge in-the-world for imperfect knowledge in-the-head , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[10]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Katsuhiko Ogata,et al.  Modern Control Engineering , 1970 .

[12]  David N. Lee,et al.  Where we look when we steer , 1994, Nature.

[13]  John R Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of the mind. , 2004, Psychological review.

[14]  Allen Newell,et al.  SOAR: An Architecture for General Intelligence , 1987, Artif. Intell..

[15]  Frank J. Lee,et al.  Does Learning a Complex Task Have to Be Complex?: A Study in Learning Decomposition , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  ACT-R/PM and menu selection: applying a cognitive architecture to HCI , 2001, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[17]  Alan D Samples,et al.  SOAR Architecture , 1985 .

[18]  D E Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.

[19]  Rob Gray,et al.  A Two-Point Visual Control Model of Steering , 2004, Perception.

[20]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[21]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  From vision to action: experiments and models of steering control during driving. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Richard Reviewer-Granger Unified Theories of Cognition , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Edmund Donges,et al.  A Two-Level Model of Driver Steering Behavior , 1978 .

[24]  Y Liu,et al.  Queueing network modeling of elementary mental processes. , 1996, Psychological review.

[25]  Julie M. Harris,et al.  Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow , 1998, Current Biology.

[26]  Dario D. Salvucci A Multitasking General Executive for Compound Continuous Tasks , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[27]  Dario D. Salvucci Predicting the effects of in-car interface use on driver performance: an integrated model approach , 2001, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[28]  Shane T. Mueller,et al.  Executive-process interactive control: A unified computational theory for answering 20 questions (and more) about cognitive ageing , 2001 .

[29]  Daniel V. McGehee,et al.  Collision Warning Timing, Driver Distraction, and Driver Response to Imminent Rear-End Collisions in a High-Fidelity Driving Simulator , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[30]  Kristen L. Macuga,et al.  Predicting the effects of cellular-phone dialing on driver performance , 2002, Cognitive Systems Research.

[31]  C. Lebiere,et al.  The Atomic Components of Thought , 1998 .

[32]  Todd Jochem,et al.  Rapidly Adapting Machine Vision for Automated Vehicle Steering , 1996, IEEE Expert.

[33]  Dario D. Salvucci Modeling Driver Distraction from Cognitive Tasks , 2019, Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[34]  E. Boer Car following from the driver’s perspective , 1999 .

[35]  Frank E. Ritter,et al.  Supporting cognitive models as users , 2000, TCHI.

[36]  Randolph M. Jones,et al.  Automated Intelligent Pilots for Combat Flight Simulation , 1998, AI Mag..

[37]  Walter W. Wierwille Development of a Strategy Model of the Driver in Lane Keeping , 1978 .

[38]  Dario D. Salvucci Inferring Driver Intent: A Case Study in Lane-Change Detection , 2004 .

[39]  Michael Land,et al.  Which parts of the road guide steering? , 1995, Nature.

[40]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Milliseconds Matter: an Introduction to Microstrategies and to Their Use in Describing and Predicting Interactive Behavior Milliseconds Matter: an Introduction to Microstrategies and to Their Use in Describing and Predicting Interactive Behavior , 2022 .

[41]  Dario D. Salvucci An integrated model of eye movements and visual encoding , 2001, Cognitive Systems Research.

[42]  A. Modjtahedzadeh,et al.  A control theoretic model of driver steering behavior , 1990, IEEE Control Systems Magazine.

[43]  John A. Michon,et al.  A critical view of driver behavior models: What do we know , 1985 .

[44]  Yili Liu,et al.  Modeling Steering Using the Queueing Network — Model Human Processor (QN-MHP) , 2003 .

[45]  Dario D. Salvucci,et al.  Distract-R: rapid prototyping and evaluation of in-vehicle interfaces , 2005, CHI.

[46]  M. Just,et al.  From the SelectedWorks of Marcel Adam Just 1992 A capacity theory of comprehension : Individual differences in working memory , 2017 .

[47]  Richard Wilkie,et al.  Controlling steering and judging heading: retinal flow, visual direction, and extraretinal information. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  J. Groeger Understanding Driving: Applying Cognitive Psychology to a Complex Everyday Task , 2000 .

[49]  H Godthelp,et al.  Vehicle Control During Curve Driving , 1986, Human factors.

[50]  Dario D. Salvucci,et al.  The time course of a lane change: Driver control and eye-movement behavior , 2002 .

[51]  Jannes Aasman,et al.  Modelling driver behaviour in soar , 1995 .

[52]  Frank J. Lee,et al.  Modeling Effects of Age in Complex Tasks: A Case Study in Driving , 2004 .

[53]  D. Kieras,et al.  Modern computational perspectives on executive mental processes and cognitive control: Where to from here? , 2000 .

[54]  Wayne D. Gray The nature and processing of errors in interactive behavior , 2000, Cogn. Sci..

[55]  William H Levison,et al.  DESCRIPTION OF THE INTEGRATED DRIVER MODEL , 1995 .