Characteristics of associative learning in younger and older adults: evidence from an episodic priming paradigm.

Two experiments investigated age differences in the encoding of associative information during a speeded naming task. In both experiments, semantically unrelated prime-target word pairs were presented 4 times, in either massed or spaced fashion, during the learning phase. An immediate or delayed test trial was presented following the fourth presentation. In Experiment 1, participants named both the primes and the targets. Younger and older adults showed similar benefits when naming targets that were part of a consistent prime-target pairing compared with targets presented with different primes at each presentation. In Experiment 2, participants named only the target word. Younger adults showed a benefit for consistently paired words, whereas older adults showed no benefit for consistently paired words. The results of the test trials showed a greater benefit for massed repeated words than for spaced repeated words at the immediate test and a reversed pattern at the delayed test. This spacing by test delay interaction was evident in response latency in Experiment 1 and in cued recall performance in Experiment 2.

[1]  J. C. Rabinowitz Priming in episodic memory. , 1986, Journal of gerontology.

[2]  J. Myerson,et al.  The information-loss model: a mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. , 1990, Psychological review.

[3]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Isolating the age deficit in speeded performance. , 1982, Journal of gerontology.

[4]  L. Light,et al.  Memory and aging: the role of retrieval processes. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  N Cowan,et al.  Implications of aging, lexicality, and item length for the mechanisms underlying memory span , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  D. Balota,et al.  Spreading Activation in Episodic Memory: Further Evidence for Age Independence , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[7]  L. Squire,et al.  On the Implicit Learning of Novel Associations by Amnesic Patients and Normal Subjects , 1993 .

[8]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  On the development of procedural knowledge. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  R A Abrams,et al.  Mental chronometry: beyond onset latencies in the lexical decision task. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  J. Fodor Psychology and Language. , 1970 .

[11]  A. Glenberg Monotonic and nonmonotonic lag effects in paired-associate and recognition memory paradigms , 1976 .

[12]  L. R. Peterson,et al.  Effect of spacing presentations on retention of a paired associate over short intervals. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  C. Fisher,et al.  The Given-New Contract in Speech to Infants , 1995 .

[14]  D. L. Hintzman Repetition and Memory1 , 1976 .

[15]  J. M. Puckett,et al.  Adult age differences in recognition memory for a nonsemantic attribute. , 1980, Experimental aging research.

[16]  Raymond J. Shaw,et al.  Aging and the priming of newly learned associations , 1986 .

[17]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[18]  Peder J. Johnson,et al.  Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure. , 1994 .

[19]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Evidence for facilitatory and inhibitory processes , 1976, Memory & cognition.

[20]  A. Hartley,et al.  Evidence for the selective preservation of spatial selective attention in old age. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[21]  Robert L. Greene,et al.  Spacing effects on implicit memory tests. , 1990 .

[22]  E. Pulgram Syllable, word, nexus, cursus , 1970 .

[23]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Theoretical implications of the spacing effect. , 1974 .

[24]  C. Fowler Differential Shortening of Repeated Content Words Produced in Various Communicative Contexts , 1988, Language and speech.

[25]  W. Rogers,et al.  Mechanisms underlying reduction in Stroop interference with practice for young and old adults. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  D. Burke,et al.  Semantic priming in young and older adults: evidence for age constancy in automatic and attentional processes. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  D. Balota,et al.  Age-related differences in lexical access, spreading activation, and simple pronunciation. , 1988, Psychology and aging.

[28]  D. Balota,et al.  Automatic and attentional priming in young and older adults: reevaluation of the two-process model. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  J. G. Gilbert Memory loss in senescence. , 1941 .

[30]  Leah L. Light,et al.  Repetition priming of nonwords in young and older adults. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  K Y Haaland,et al.  Skill learning in the elderly: diminished implicit and explicit memory for a motor sequence. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[32]  M Moscovitch,et al.  Memory as assessed by recognition and reading time in normal and memory-impaired people with Alzheimer's disease and other neurological disorders. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Priming in episodic and semantic memory. , 1979 .

[34]  D. Howard,et al.  Age differences in learning serial patterns: direct versus indirect measures. , 1989, Psychology and aging.

[35]  J Cerella,et al.  Age effects may be global, not local: comment on Fisk and Rogers (1991) , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[36]  D. Balota,et al.  Age-related differences in the impact of spacing, lag, and retention interval. , 1989, Psychology and aging.

[37]  D. Balota,et al.  Repetition and Associative Context Effects in Speech Production , 1991, Language and speech.

[38]  C. Fowler,et al.  Talkers' signaling of new and old. words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction , 1987 .

[39]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Psychology and language : an introduction to psycholinguistics , 1979 .

[40]  R M Shiffrin,et al.  Episodic and lexical contributions to the repetition effect in word identification. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[41]  Bradford H. Challis,et al.  Spacing effects on cued-memory tests depend on level of processing. , 1993 .

[42]  D. Balota,et al.  Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson′s Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation , 1993, Brain and Cognition.

[43]  D. Balota,et al.  Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  D. G. MacKay,et al.  Chapter Five Cognition and Aging: A Theory of New Learning and the Use of Old Connections , 1990 .

[45]  Robert L. Greene,et al.  Spacing effects in memory: evidence for a two-process account , 1989 .

[46]  D. Balota,et al.  Depth of Automatic Spreading Activation: Mediated Priming Effects in Pronunciation but Not in Lexical Decision , 1986 .

[47]  A. Fry,et al.  Aging and memory for new associations: direct versus indirect measures. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[48]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[49]  Julie E. Boland,et al.  Priming in pronunciation: Beyond pattern recognition and onset latency , 1989 .

[50]  R. Monge Studies of verbal learning from the college years through middle age. , 1971, Journal of gerontology.