Changing Simple Energy-Related Consumer Behaviors

Achieving ambitious CO2 emission reductions require changing mundane consumer behaviors in addition to increasing modern technology’s energy-saving potential. Frequently, energy-related behaviors, however, may occur highly and thus tend to become habitual. On basis of the notion that habits represent automatic reactions to situations, and hence, are hard to control, we examined whether the capability to perform or abstain from performing actions depends on the type of habit established. We tested whether an acting habit (switching off the light upon leaving a room) supports execution of the intention to act (switching it off), but hinders the intention to abstain from acting (not switching it off). Conversely, a habit of not switching off the light (a non-acting habit) is expected to obstruct execution of the intention to switch the light off, but facilitates the intention to not switch it off. Results support these ideas, and underscore the importance of differentiating between habits resulting from frequently acting and frequently not acting.

[1]  Horace Herring,et al.  Energy efficiency—a critical view , 2006 .

[2]  H. Aarts,et al.  Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed behavior. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  C. Knussen,et al.  An analysis of intentions to recycle household waste: The roles of past behaviour, perceived habit, and perceived lack of facilities , 2004 .

[4]  D. Wegner Ironic processes of mental control. , 1994, Psychological review.

[5]  J. Bargh,et al.  Environmental control of goal-directed action: automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. , 1994, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[6]  C. Midden,et al.  To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors , 1999 .

[7]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[8]  M. DenticeD'accadia,et al.  Energy Efficiency in Household Appliances and Lighting , 2001 .

[9]  P. Gollwitzer Goal Achievement: The Role of Intentions , 1993 .

[10]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[11]  H. Aarts,et al.  THE AUTOMATIC ACTIVATION OF GOAL-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR: THE CASE OF TRAVEL HABIT , 2000 .

[12]  P. Schmidt,et al.  Incentives, Morality, Or Habit? Predicting Students’ Car Use for University Routes With the Models of Ajzen, Schwartz, and Triandis , 2003 .

[13]  A. Graybiel The Basal Ganglia and Chunking of Action Repertoires , 1998, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[14]  Daniel T. Gilbert,et al.  Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. , 1989 .

[15]  H. Heckhausen,et al.  Intentional action and action slips. , 1990 .

[16]  R. Gifford Environmental Psychology: Principles and Practice , 1987 .

[17]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound , 1994 .

[18]  H. Aarts,et al.  Physical exercise habit: on the conceptualization and formation of habitual health behaviours. , 1997, Health education research.

[19]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .

[20]  Jeffrey M. Quinn,et al.  Habits in everyday life: thought, emotion, and action. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Henry L. Tosi A Theory of Goal Setting and Task Performance , 1991 .

[22]  B. Verplanken,et al.  Predicting behavior from actions in the past : repeated decision making or a matter of habit? , 1998 .

[23]  Banwari Mittal Achieving Higher Seat Belt Usage: The Role of Habit in Bridging the Attitude‐Behavior Gap1 , 1988 .

[24]  D. Wegner,et al.  Control and automaticity in social life. , 1998 .

[25]  Sebastian Bamberg,et al.  EFFECTS OF IMPLEMENTATION INTENTIONS ON THE ACTUAL PERFORMANCE OF NEW ENVIRONMENTALLY FRIENDLY BEHAVIOURS — RESULTS OF TWO FIELD EXPERIMENTS , 2002 .

[26]  D. Ronis,et al.  Attitudes, decisions, and habits as determinants of repeated behavior. , 1989 .

[27]  C. I. Connolly,et al.  Building neural representations of habits. , 1999, Science.

[28]  J. Reason Actions not as planned : The price of automatisation , 1979 .

[29]  C. Carver,et al.  Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior , 1981 .

[30]  C. Vlek,et al.  A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation , 2005 .

[31]  J. T. Austin,et al.  Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. , 1996 .

[32]  H C Triandis,et al.  Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. , 1980, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[33]  J. Bargh The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. , 1994 .

[34]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. , 1987 .

[35]  Wendy Wood,et al.  Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. , 1998 .

[36]  Henk Aarts,et al.  Breaking and creating habits on the working floor : A field- experiment on the power of implementation intentions , 2006 .

[37]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit , 1997 .

[38]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[39]  S. Bamberg Is a Residential Relocation a Good Opportunity to Change People’s Travel Behavior? Results From a Theory-Driven Intervention Study , 2006 .

[40]  H. Aarts,et al.  Habit, Action, and Consciousness , 2009 .

[41]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures , 1978 .

[42]  P. Sheeran,et al.  How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes , 2007 .

[43]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Attitude structure and function. , 1998 .

[44]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[45]  B. Verplanken,et al.  Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength , 2003 .

[46]  B. Roche,et al.  The Behavior of Organisms? , 1997 .

[47]  John R. Anderson Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition , 2013 .

[48]  William P. Banks,et al.  Encyclopedia of consciousness , 2009 .

[49]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[50]  B. Verplanken,et al.  Habit, attitude, and planned behaviour : is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity? , 1999 .

[51]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Memory and Cognition in Its Social Context , 1989 .

[52]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes , 2006 .

[53]  C. Klöckner,et al.  How habits interfere with norm-directed behaviour: A normative decision-making model for travel mode choice , 2004 .

[54]  C. Klöckner,et al.  Travel Mode Choice of Women , 2002 .

[55]  Sarah C. Darby,et al.  Making it Obvious: Designing Feedback into Energy Consumption , 2001 .

[56]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[57]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Implementation Intentions and the Theory of Planned Behavior , 1997, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[58]  D. Norman Categorization of action slips. , 1981 .