Cognitive Predictors of Cortical Thickness in Healthy Aging.

This study seeks to define the role of predictive values of the motor speed, inhibition control, and fluid and crystallized intelligence in estimating the cortical thickness in healthy elderly. Forty-six older healthy subjects (37 women, 9 men) over 60 years of age were included in the study. The participants were examined on 3.0 T MRI scanners. The protocol included standard anatomical sequences, to exclude brain pathology, and a high-resolution T1-weighted sequence used to estimate the cortical thickness. The neuropsychological protocol included fluid intelligence assessment (Raven Progressive Matrices), crystalized intelligence assessment (information or vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R)), and executive functioning (Color Traits Test). The findings unraveled several interdependencies. The higher the intelligence, the thicker was the grey matter in nine regions of both hemispheres, but also some paradoxical reversed associations were found in four areas; all of them were localized along different sections of the cingulate gyrus in both hemispheres. An inverse association was found between crystallized intelligence and the thickness of the pars opecularis of the right hemisphere. The better the executive functioning, the thicker was the grey matter of a given region. The better the motor performance, the thicker was the grey matter of the rostral middle frontal area of the left hemisphere and the lingual gyrus of both hemispheres. In conclusion, the associations unraveled demonstrate that the neural mechanisms underlying healthy aging are complex and heterogenic across different cognitive domains and neuroanatomical regions. No brain aging theory seems to provide a suitable interpretative framework for all the results. A novel, more integrative approach to the brain aging should be considered.

[1]  Nicolas Cherbuin,et al.  Cortical gyrification and its relationships with cortical volume, cortical thickness, and cognitive performance in healthy mid-life adults , 2015, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  Peng Yuan,et al.  Regional brain shrinkage and change in cognitive performance over two years: The bidirectional influences of the brain and cognitive reserve factors , 2016, NeuroImage.

[3]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Trajectories of brain aging in middle-aged and older adults: Regional and individual differences , 2010, NeuroImage.

[4]  Qolamreza R. Razlighi,et al.  Selective association between cortical thickness and reference abilities in normal aging , 2016, NeuroImage.

[5]  R. Swerdlow Is aging part of Alzheimer's disease, or is Alzheimer's disease part of aging? , 2007, Neurobiology of Aging.

[6]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Genetic and Lifestyle Predictors of 15‐Year Longitudinal Change in Episodic Memory , 2012, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[7]  M. Freedman,et al.  Quantifying qualitative features of Block Design performance among healthy older adults. , 2001, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[8]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Selective increase of cortical thickness in high-performing elderly—structural indices of optimal cognitive aging , 2006, NeuroImage.

[9]  J. Chaplin Dictionary of psychology , 1975 .

[10]  Shelley Channon,et al.  Humor processing, mentalizing, and executive function in normal aging , 2006, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[11]  A. Dale,et al.  High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Arno Klein,et al.  101 Labeled Brain Images and a Consistent Human Cortical Labeling Protocol , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[13]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain connectivity in normally developing children and adolescents , 2016, NeuroImage.

[14]  Monica Luciana,et al.  Associations between cortical thickness and general intelligence in children, adolescents and young adults. , 2013, Intelligence.

[15]  P. Thompson,et al.  Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[16]  R. Palmer,et al.  Normal rates of cognitive change in successful aging: The Freedom House Study , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[17]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images , 2004, NeuroImage.

[18]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  A comparison between voxel-based cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in normal aging , 2009, NeuroImage.

[19]  A. Dale,et al.  One-Year Brain Atrophy Evident in Healthy Aging , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.

[23]  George Richardson,et al.  Brain development and aging: Overlapping and unique patterns of change , 2013, NeuroImage.

[24]  A. Dale,et al.  Brain Changes in Older Adults at Very Low Risk for Alzheimer's Disease , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  S. Resnick,et al.  Longitudinal Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Older Adults: A Shrinking Brain , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Denise C. Park,et al.  The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[27]  T. Salthouse Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[28]  A. Wunderlich,et al.  Variability in memory performance in aged healthy individuals: an fMRI study , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[29]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest , 2006, NeuroImage.

[30]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. , 2010, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[31]  Nikos Makris,et al.  Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Aaron Carass,et al.  Longitudinal changes in cortical thickness associated with normal aging , 2010, NeuroImage.

[33]  A. Dale,et al.  Accelerating cortical thinning: unique to dementia or universal in aging? , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Cheryl L. Dahle,et al.  Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  N. Raz,et al.  Differential Aging of the Brain: Patterns, Cognitive Correlates and Modifiers , 2022 .

[36]  P. Matthews,et al.  Risk factors for progression of brain atrophy in aging , 2005, Neurology.

[37]  N. Pedersen,et al.  Age changes in processing speed as a leading indicator of cognitive aging. , 2007, Psychology and aging.

[38]  H. Braak,et al.  Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes , 2004, Acta Neuropathologica.

[39]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Human brain development over the early years , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[40]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Cortical thickness asymmetry from childhood to older adulthood , 2013, NeuroImage.

[41]  R. Cabeza Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[42]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[43]  Badiâa Bouazzaoui,et al.  Aging and strategic retrieval in a cued-recall test: The role of executive functions and fluid intelligence , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[44]  R. Cattell Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. , 1963 .

[45]  A. Dale,et al.  High‐resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface , 1999, Human brain mapping.