Beyond the Flesh: Some Lessons from a Mole Cricket
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Lewkowicz,et al. A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. , 2007, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.
[2] R. K. Thompson,et al. Language-naive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) judge relations between relations in a conceptual matching-to-sample task. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[3] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[4] J. S. Turner,et al. The Extended Organism: The Physiology of Animal-Built Structures , 2000 .
[5] R. Cox. Representation construction, externalised cognition and individual differences , 1999 .
[6] Randall D. Beer,et al. Further Experiments in the Evolution of Minimally Cognitive Behavior: From Perceiving Affordances to Selective Attention , 2000 .
[7] T. Gelder,et al. The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[8] Alan Garfinkel,et al. Self-organizing systems : the emergence of order , 1987 .
[9] Leslie Marsh. Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence , 2009 .
[10] J. Lucy,et al. Language acquisition and conceptual development: Grammatical categories and the development of classification preferences: a comparative approach , 2001 .
[11] A. Clark. Leadership and Influence: The Manager as Coach, Nanny and Artificial DNA , 1999 .
[12] Judy S. DeLoache,et al. Symbolic functioning in preschool children , 1994 .
[13] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Embodiment is the foundation, not a level , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[14] Andy Clark,et al. Doing without representing? , 1994, Synthese.
[15] Henrik Gedenryd. How designers work - making sense of authentic cognitive activities , 1998 .
[16] R. M. Gaze. Dynamic patterns , 1975, Nature.
[17] K. Cheng. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.
[18] Pattie Maes,et al. Toward the Evolution of Dynamical Neural Networks for Minimally Cognitive Behavior , 1996 .
[19] D. Knight. Kinds of minds , 2007, Nature.
[20] M K Kaiser,et al. How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. , 1995, Science.
[21] Robert L. Goldstone,et al. The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[22] Elizabeth S Spelke,et al. Language and number: a bilingual training study , 2001, Cognition.
[23] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.
[24] Randall D. Beer,et al. Biological Neural Networks in Invertebrate Neuroethology and Robotics. Editors: Randall D. Beer, Roy E. Ritzmann, Thomas McKenna (Academic Press, Inc., Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1993) , 1996, SGAR.
[25] Robert L. Goldstone,et al. The development of features in object concepts , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[26] D. Gentner,et al. Structural Alignment during Similarity Comparisons , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.
[27] Kevin Warwick. Natural-born cyborgs: Minds, technologies and the future of human intelligence , 2003 .
[28] E. Spelke,et al. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.
[29] Maja J. Mataric,et al. Integration of representation into goal-driven behavior-based robots , 1992, IEEE Trans. Robotics Autom..
[30] T. Gelder,et al. What Might Cognition Be, If Not Computation? , 1995 .
[31] J. Deloache. Symbolic functioning in very young children: understanding of pictures and models. , 1991, Child development.
[32] Rolf Pfeifer,et al. Understanding intelligence , 2020, Inequality by Design.
[33] A. Clark. Magic Words: How Language Augments Human Computation , 1998 .
[34] J. Cacioppo,et al. Quantity-based interference and symbolic representations in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[35] Andy Clark,et al. Natural-Born Cyborgs? , 2001, Cognitive Technology.
[36] F. Varela,et al. Radical embodiment: neural dynamics and consciousness , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[37] Seungjin Choi,et al. Shaping meanings for language: universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semanti , 2001 .
[38] De Vries. Book review: R.C. O'Reilly and Y. Munakata: Computational explorations in cognitive neuroscience: understanding the mind by stimulating the brain. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press. , 2002 .
[39] R. O’Reilly,et al. Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain , 2000 .
[40] L. Barsalou,et al. Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[41] Yvonne Rogers,et al. External cognition: how do graphical representations work? , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..
[42] R. Gregory,et al. Mind in Science. A History of Explanations in Psychology and Physics , 1983 .
[43] E. Thelen,et al. The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[44] Linda B. Smith. Language acquisition and conceptual development: How domain-general processes may create domain-specific biases , 2001 .
[45] G. Lakoff,et al. Philosophy in the flesh : the embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought , 1999 .
[46] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.
[47] Carl Anderson,et al. The extended organism: The physiology of animal-built structures , 2000, Complex..
[48] Andy Clark. Forces, Fields, and the Role of Knowledge in Action , 2003, Adapt. Behav..
[49] P. Carruthers. The cognitive functions of language , 2002, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[50] Barbara Webb,et al. A Cricket Robot , 1996 .
[51] Andy Clark,et al. Visual Awareness and Visuomotor Action , 1999 .