Are there right hemisphere contributions to visually-guided movement? Manipulating left hand reaction time advantages in dextrals

Many studies have argued for distinct but complementary contributions from each hemisphere in the control of movements to visual targets. Investigators have attempted to extend observations from patients with unilateral left- and right-hemisphere damage, to those using neurologically-intact participants, by assuming that each hand has privileged access to the contralateral hemisphere. Previous attempts to illustrate right hemispheric contributions to the control of aiming have focussed on increasing the spatial demands of an aiming task, to attenuate the typical right hand advantages, to try to enhance a left hand reaction time advantage in right-handed participants. These early attempts have not been successful. The present study circumnavigates some of the theoretical and methodological difficulties of some of the earlier experiments, by using three different tasks linked directly to specialized functions of the right hemisphere: bisecting, the gap effect, and visuospatial localization. None of these tasks were effective in reducing the magnitude of left hand reaction time advantages in right handers. Results are discussed in terms of alternatives to right hemispheric functional explanations of the effect, the one-dimensional nature of our target arrays, power and precision given the size of the left hand RT effect, and the utility of examining the proportions of participants who show these effects, rather than exclusive reliance on measures of central tendency and their associated null hypothesis significance tests.

[1]  D. Carey,et al.  Quantifying cerebral asymmetries for language in dextrals and adextrals with random-effects meta analysis , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[2]  Kenneth M. Heilman,et al.  Apraxia : The Neuropsychology of Action , 2014 .

[3]  G. Goldenberg Apraxia – The cognitive side of motor control , 2013, Cortex.

[4]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Complementary hemispheric specialization for language production and visuospatial attention , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  W. Poynter,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries in visual search , 2012, Laterality.

[6]  Daniel Voyer,et al.  Free-viewing laterality tasks: a multilevel meta-analysis. , 2012, Neuropsychology.

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  M. Coltheart What has functional neuroimaging told us about the organization of mind? , 2011 .

[9]  G. Cumming Understanding the New Statistics: Effect Sizes, Confidence Intervals, and Meta-Analysis , 2011 .

[10]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Three-dimensional transformations for goal-directed action. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  J. Pratt,et al.  Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Right Hemisphere Dominance during Spatial Selective Attention and Target Detection Occurs Outside the Dorsal Frontoparietal Network , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Joseph R. Rausch,et al.  Sample size planning for statistical power and accuracy in parameter estimation. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  A. Compston Visual disorientation with special reference to lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. By W. Russell Brain (From the Neurological Department, London Hospital) Brain 1941: 64; 244–272. , 2007 .

[15]  F. Vitu,et al.  On the limited role of target onset in the gap task: support for the motor-preparation hypothesis. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[16]  Johan Wagemans,et al.  Texture and object motion in slant discrimination: failure of reliability-based weighting of cues may be evidence for strong fusion. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[17]  S. Ferber,et al.  Revisiting unilateral neglect , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  G. Michael,et al.  Visual field asymmetries in selective attention: Evidence from a modified search paradigm , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  M. Nicholls,et al.  The greyscales task: a perceptual measure of attentional bias following unilateral hemispheric damage , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Mieke Verfaellie,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries for selective attention apparent only with increased task demands in healthy participants , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Handedness: dominant arm advantages in control of limb dynamics. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Orienting visuospatial attention generates manual reaction time asymmetries in target detection and pointing , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[23]  Richard P. DeShon,et al.  Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[24]  D. Carey,et al.  Hemispatial differences in visually guided aiming are neither hemispatial nor visual , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  V. Nougier,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Reaching Movement Control. I: Study of Right-Handers , 2001, Cortex.

[26]  V. Nougier,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Reaching Movement Control. II: Study of Left-Handers , 2001, Cortex.

[27]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Complementary localization and lateralization of orienting and motor attention , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Goal-Directed Movements , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[29]  Jennifer S. Beer,et al.  Hemispheric Dominance for Emotions, Empathy and Social Behaviour: Evidence from Right and Left Handers with Frontotemporal Dementia. , 2001, Neurocase.

[30]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Influence of arm movements on saccades in humans , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[31]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  R. Fendrich,et al.  The oculomotor gap effect without a foveal fixation point , 1999, Vision Research.

[34]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.

[35]  M. Atienza,et al.  The gap effect during visual and auditory stimulation using manual responses , 1998, Biological Psychology.

[36]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Reaching to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  D. Harrington,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of movement , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Motor Performance , 1996 .

[39]  H. Bekkering,et al.  The gap effect for eye and hand movements , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[40]  W. Dunlap,et al.  Meta-Analysis of Experiments With Matched Groups or Repeated Measures Designs , 1996 .

[41]  J. M. Miller,et al.  Egocentric localization of a perisaccadic flash by manual pointing , 1996, Vision Research.

[42]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Preparation of Aiming Movements , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[43]  D. Munoz,et al.  A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Richard T. Marrocco,et al.  Arousal systems , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[45]  D. Elliott,et al.  Kinematic Analyses of Manual Asymmetries in Visual Aiming Movements , 1994, Brain and Cognition.

[46]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of intermittent vision on manual aiming. , 1994, Acta psychologica.

[47]  R. Klein,et al.  Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: does predisengagement of visuospatial attention mediate this gap effect? , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  Romeo Chua,et al.  The role of impulse variability in manual-aiming asymmetries , 1993 .

[49]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Manual Aiming Movements , 1993 .

[50]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[51]  R. Klein,et al.  What are human express saccades? , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[52]  A. Milner,et al.  Line bisection errors in visual neglect: Misguided action or size distortion? , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Asymmetries in the spatial localization of transformed targets , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[54]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the discrete and pseudocontinuous regulation of visually guided reaching , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[55]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  The reduction of saccadic latency by prior offset of the fixation point: An analysis of the gap effect , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[56]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Manual asymmetries in the reproduction of a 3-dimensional spatial location , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  A. Lerner,et al.  Neurotrauma , 1990, Neurology.

[58]  M A Goodale,et al.  Kinematic analysis of limb movements in neuropsychological research: subtle deficits and recovery of function. , 1990, Canadian journal of psychology.

[59]  O. Tzeng,et al.  Cerebral asymmetry in visual attention , 1990, Brain and Cognition.

[60]  Doreen Kimura,et al.  Right-hand superiority for throwing but not for intercepting , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  D. Elliott,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Aimed Movements , 1989 .

[62]  M. P. Bryden,et al.  Different Dimensions of Hand Preference That Relate to Skilled and Unskilled Activities , 1989, Cortex.

[63]  D. Harrington,et al.  The role of the hemispheres in closed loop movements , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[64]  M. Goodale Hemispheric differences in motor control , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[65]  Yves Guiard,et al.  Left-hand advantage in right-handers for spatial constant error: Preliminary evidence in a unimanual ballistic aimed movement , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[66]  J Paillard,et al.  Apraxia and the neurophysiology of motor control. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[67]  T. Schenkenberg,et al.  Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment , 1980, Neurology.

[68]  B Milner,et al.  THE ROLE OF EARLY LEFT‐BRAIN INJURY IN DETERMINING LATERALIZATION OF CEREBRAL SPEECH FUNCTIONS , 1977, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[69]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  R. Kirk Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences , 1970 .

[71]  D. Kimura,et al.  Spatial localization in left and right visual fields. , 1969, Canadian journal of psychology.

[72]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[73]  W. R. Brain VISUAL DISORIENTATION WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO LESIONS OF THE RIGHT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE , 1941 .

[74]  Diana Adler,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 2016 .

[75]  J. Bermúdez,et al.  The organization of the mind , 2013 .

[76]  M. Goodale,et al.  The effects of unilateral brain damage on visually guided reaching: hemispheric differences in the nature of the deficit , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[77]  J. Findlay,et al.  Express saccades: is there a separate population in humans? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[78]  O. Bock,et al.  Contribution of retinal versus extraretinal signals towards visual localization in goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[79]  J. D. Fisk,et al.  The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[80]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[81]  Rex B. Kline,et al.  Beyond Significance Testing: Reforming Data Analysis Methods in Behavioral Research , 2004 .

[82]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Estimating the components of the gap effect , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[83]  L. Elias,et al.  Footedness is a better predictor of language lateralisation than handedness. , 1998, Laterality.

[84]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of right hemisphere neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998 .

[85]  Doreen Kimura,et al.  Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication , 1993 .

[86]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Is Cognitive Neuropsychology Plausible? The Perils of Sitting on a One-Legged Stool , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[87]  R. Carson Chapter 3 Visual Feedback Processing and Manual Asymmetries: An Evolving Perspective , 1992 .

[88]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[89]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Vision and Action: The Control of Grasping , 1990 .

[90]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[91]  E. Renzi Disorders of space exploration and cognition , 1982 .

[92]  D. Kimura,et al.  Motor functions of the left hemisphere. , 1974, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[93]  Herbert E. Hendry On reduction , 1969 .