Concurrent validity of approximate number sense tasks in adults and children.

Reasoning with non-symbolic numerosities is suggested to be rooted in the Approximate Number System (ANS) and evidence pointing to a relationship between the acuity of this system and mathematics is available. In order to use the acuity of this ANS as a screening instrument to detect future math problems, it is important to model ANS acuity over development. However, whether ANS acuity and its development have been described accurately can be questioned. Namely, different tasks were used to examine the developmental trajectory of ANS acuity and studies comparing performances on these different tasks are scarce. In the present study, we examined whether different tasks designed to measure the acuity of the ANS are comparable and lead to related ANS acuity measures (i.e., the concurrent validity of these tasks). We contrasted the change detection task, which is used in infants, with tasks that are more commonly used in older children and adults (i.e., comparison and same-different tasks). Together, our results suggest that ANS acuity measures obtained with different tasks are not related. This poses serious problems for the comparison of ANS acuity measures derived from different tasks and thus for the establishment of the developmental trajectory of ANS acuity.

[1]  L. Lagae,et al.  Developmental changes in visuo-spatial working memory in normally developing children: Event-related potentials study , 2013, Brain and Development.

[2]  Mieke Vandewaetere,et al.  What can the same-different task tell us about the development of magnitude representations? , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[3]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Indexing the approximate number system. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[4]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[5]  Emmy Defever,et al.  The Approximate Number System is not Predictive for Symbolic Number Processing in Kindergarteners , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[7]  Kenneth A. Bollen,et al.  Regression Diagnostics , 1985 .

[8]  P. G. Vos,et al.  A probabilistic model for the discrimination of visual number , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[10]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[11]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[12]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[13]  Emmy Defever,et al.  Task- and age-dependent effects of visual stimulus properties on children's explicit numerosity judgments. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Sampling from the mental number line: How are approximate number system representations formed? , 2013, Cognition.

[15]  E. Spelke,et al.  The construction of large number representations in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[16]  Michaël A. Stevens,et al.  A model of exact small-number representation , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[18]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Measuring the approximate number system in children: Exploring the relationships among different tasks , 2014 .

[19]  S. Luck,et al.  The development of visual short-term memory capacity in infants. , 2003, Child development.

[20]  N. Prins Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction , 2009 .

[21]  G. Fechner Elemente der Psychophysik , 1998 .

[22]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[23]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[24]  J. Tanji,et al.  Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey , 2002, Nature.

[25]  E. Van den Bussche,et al.  The reliability of and the relation between non-symbolic numerical distance effects in comparison, same-different judgments and priming. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[26]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  Russell Gersten,et al.  Early Identification and Interventions for Students With Mathematics Difficulties , 2005, Journal of learning disabilities.

[28]  Fei Xu,et al.  Number discrimination in 10-month-old infants , 2007 .

[29]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[30]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  Amy Devine,et al.  Visual stimulus parameters seriously compromise the measurement of approximate number system acuity and comparative effects between adults and children , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[32]  Chaitanya Ramineni,et al.  Predicting First–Grade Math Achievement from Developmental Number Sense Trajectories , 2007 .

[33]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: A developmental study , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[34]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Measuring the Approximate Number System , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Representation of the Quantity of Visual Items in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 2002, Science.

[36]  N. Macmillan,et al.  The psychophysics of categorical perception. , 1977, Psychological review.

[37]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[38]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli , 2011, Behavior research methods.

[39]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[40]  T. Verguts,et al.  The origins of the numerical distance effect: The same–different task , 2011 .

[41]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Comparing the neural distance effect derived from the non–symbolic comparison and the same–different task , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[42]  Emmy Defever,et al.  Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[43]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The representation of numerical magnitude , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[44]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[45]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Mental representation: What can pitch tell us about the distance effect? , 2008, Cortex.

[46]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. , 2010, Developmental science.

[47]  Titia Gebuis,et al.  False Approximations of the Approximate Number System? , 2011, PloS one.

[48]  Ariel Starr,et al.  Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.