Brain activity dynamics in human parietal regions during spontaneous switches in bistable perception

The neural mechanisms underlying conscious visual perception have been extensively investigated using bistable perception paradigms. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies suggest that the right anterior superior parietal (r-aSPL) and the right posterior superior parietal lobule (r-pSPL) have opposite roles in triggering perceptual reversals. It has been proposed that these two areas are part of a hierarchical network whose dynamics determine perceptual switches. However, how these two parietal regions interact with each other and with the rest of the brain during bistable perception is not known. Here, we investigated such a model by recording brain activity using fMRI while participants viewed a bistable structure-from-motion stimulus. Using dynamic causal modeling (DCM), we found that resolving such perceptual ambiguity was specifically associated with reciprocal interactions between these parietal regions and V5/MT. Strikingly, the strength of bottom-up coupling between V5/MT to r-pSPL and from r-pSPL to r-aSPL predicted individual mean dominance duration. Our findings are consistent with a hierarchical predictive coding model of parietal involvement in bistable perception and suggest that visual information processing underlying spontaneous perceptual switches can be described as changes in connectivity strength between parietal and visual cortical regions.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[2]  V. Walsh,et al.  Right parietal TMS shortens dominance durations in binocular rivalry , 2010, Current Biology.

[3]  M. Greicius,et al.  Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. , 1999 .

[5]  G. Rees,et al.  The Neural Bases of Multistable Perception , 2022 .

[6]  E. Freeman,et al.  fMRI correlates of subjective reversals in ambiguous structure-from-motion. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysing connectivity with Granger causality and dynamic causal modelling , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[8]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Hierarchical Model of Binocular Rivalry , 1998, Neural Computation.

[9]  Aaron Schurger A very inexpensive MRI-compatible method for dichoptic visual stimulation , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[10]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of the contents of visual awareness in humans , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing dynamic causal models , 2004, NeuroImage.

[13]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing Families of Dynamic Causal Models , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[14]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A new anatomical landmark for reliable identification of human area V5/MT: a quantitative analysis of sulcal patterning. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Structural and functional fractionation of right superior parietal cortex in bistable perception , 2011, Current Biology.

[16]  G. Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. , 1998, Science.

[17]  Randolph Blake,et al.  The Role of Frontal and Parietal Brain Areas in Bistable Perception , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling , 2010, NeuroImage.

[19]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Disrupting Parietal Function Prolongs Dominance Durations in Binocular Rivalry , 2010, Current Biology.

[20]  C. Segebarth,et al.  Identifying Neural Drivers with Functional MRI: An Electrophysiological Validation , 2008, PLoS biology.

[21]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Diffusion-Weighted Imaging Tractography-Based Parcellation of the Human Parietal Cortex and Comparison with Human and Macaque Resting-State Functional Connectivity , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Robert Turner,et al.  Image Distortion Correction in fMRI: A Quantitative Evaluation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[23]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  Peter Zeidman,et al.  Identifying Abnormal Connectivity in Patients Using Dynamic Causal Modeling of fMRI Responses , 2010, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[25]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding explains binocular rivalry: An epistemological review , 2008, Cognition.

[27]  R. S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Human brain activity during spontaneously reversing perception of ambiguous figures , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  Philipp Sterzer,et al.  A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  Lars Muckli,et al.  Primary Visual Cortex Activity along the Apparent-Motion Trace Reflects Illusory Perception , 2005, PLoS biology.

[30]  A. Clark Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[31]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian model selection for group studies , 2009, NeuroImage.

[32]  Paul Schrater,et al.  Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  Daniel Arteaga,et al.  Brain mechanisms for simple perception and bistable perception , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  R. Dennis Cook,et al.  Detection of Influential Observation in Linear Regression , 2000, Technometrics.

[35]  Philipp Sterzer,et al.  Frontoparietal Cortex Mediates Perceptual Transitions in Bistable Perception , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain , 2007, Nature.

[37]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  Predictive Codes for Forthcoming Perception in the Frontal Cortex , 2006, Science.

[38]  Rachel N. Denison,et al.  Predictive Context Influences Perceptual Selection during Binocular Rivalry , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[39]  W. Einhäuser,et al.  Binocular Rivalry: Frontal Activity Relates to Introspection and Action But Not to Perception , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.