Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information

Several studies have shown that adults integrate visual and haptic information (and information from other modalities) in a statistically optimal fashion, weighting each sense according to its reliability [1, 2]. When does this capacity for crossmodal integration develop? Here, we show that prior to 8 years of age, integration of visual and haptic spatial information is far from optimal, with either vision or touch dominating totally, even in conditions in which the dominant sense is far less precise than the other (assessed by discrimination thresholds). For size discrimination, haptic information dominates in determining both perceived size and discrimination thresholds, whereas for orientation discrimination, vision dominates. By 8-10 years, the integration becomes statistically optimal, like adults. We suggest that during development, perceptual systems require constant recalibration, for which cross-sensory comparison is important. Using one sense to calibrate the other precludes useful combination of the two sources.

[1]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Comparison of sensitivity to first- and second-order local motion in 5-year-olds and adults. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[2]  E. Hartmann Perceptual Development: Visual, Auditory, and Speech Perception in Infancy , 1999 .

[3]  Mark T. Wallace,et al.  Chapter 8 The visually responsive neuron and beyond: multisensory integration in cat and monkey , 1993 .

[4]  Wayne A. Hershberger,et al.  Haptic estimates of discordant visual—haptic size vary developmentally , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Development of multisensory spatial integration and perception in humans. , 2006, Developmental science.

[6]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  Integration of shape information from vision and touch: Optimal perception and neural correlates , 2010 .

[7]  G. Berkeley Essay Towards a New Theory of Vision , 2004 .

[8]  O. Braddick,et al.  A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams' syndrome , 1997, Neuroreport.

[9]  A. Streri,et al.  Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes in human newborns , 2003, Somatosensory & motor research.

[10]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[11]  C. Granrud,et al.  Development of size constancy in children: A test of the proximal mode sensitivity hypothesis , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  Terry Caelli,et al.  Development of configural 3D object recognition , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional analysis of primary visual cortex (V1) in humans. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  I. Kovács,et al.  Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  H. McGurk,et al.  Intermodal coordination in young children: Vision and touch. , 1980 .

[17]  H. Leibowitz,et al.  Apparent visual size as a function of distance for children and adults. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.

[18]  B E Stein,et al.  Sequence of changes in properties of neurons of superior colliculus of the kitten during maturation. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  The Developing Visual Brain , 2000 .

[20]  A. Slater Perceptual Development: Visual, Auditory, and Speech Perception in Infancy. Studies in Developmental Psychology. , 2000 .

[21]  M. Ernst,et al.  Optimal integration of shape information from vision and touch , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[23]  Yvette Hatwell,et al.  Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Hand in Infancy and Childhood , 1987 .

[24]  M. Wallace,et al.  Sensory and Multisensory Responses in the Newborn Monkey Superior Colliculus , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  M T Wallace,et al.  The visually responsive neuron and beyond: multisensory integration in cat and monkey. , 1993, Progress in brain research.