Semantic and associative relations in adolescents and young adults: Examining a tenuous dichotomy.

The constructs of semantic and associative relatedness have played a prominent role in research on semantic memory because researchers have historically drawn on the distinction between these two types of relations when formulating theories, creating experimental conditions, and explaining empirical results. We argue that the binary distinction between semantics and association is rooted in a fundamental problem in how the two are defined and contrasted. Whereas semantic relatedness has typically been limited to category coordinates, associative relatedness has most often been operationalized using the word association task. We show that meaningful semantic relations between words/concepts certainly extend beyond category coordinates, that word association is driven primarily by meaningful semantic relations between cue and response words, and that non-meaningful, purely associative relations between words generally are not retained in memory. To illustrate these points, we discuss research on semantic priming, picture naming, and the Deese-Roediger-McDermott false memory paradigm. Furthermore, we describe how research on the development of mnemonic skills in adolescents supports our view. That is, adolescents do not learn arbitrary associations between words, but develop elaborative strategies for linking words by drawing on their rich knowledge of events and situations. In other words, adolescents use existing memories of meaningful relations to ground their memories for novel word pairs, even in an associative learning paradigm. The term “semantic memory” is used to refer to people’s memory for concepts and word meanings. An important aspect of understanding semantic memory concerns delineating the ways in which knowledge of word meaning is organized, and as such, a great deal of research has been aimed at providing insight into this issue. A key goal in this regard is to uncover the relations among concepts that are encoded in semantic memory. To this end, the constructs of semantic and associative relations have been central components of theories of the organization of semantic memory, and research comparing the two has provided a substantial amount of informative data that have furthered both theory development and empirical work. However, critical issues remain with regard to how semantic and associative relations have been defined and studied in semantic memory research, and how they might best be defined and studied in future research. In an influential paper on the organization of human memory, Tulving (1972) noted an increased interest among some of his contemporaries in the kind of memory that underlies the seemingly effortless execution of skills such as language processing and memory access. Tulving’s definition of semantic memory still nicely captures some commonly held views: Semantic memory is the memory necessary for the use of language. It is a mental thesaurus, organized knowledge a person possesses about words and other verbal symbols, their meaning and referents, about relations among them, and about rules, formulas, and algorithms for the manipulation of these symbols, concepts, and relations. (p. 386) Tulving also stated that “the relations among items in semantic memory are of much greater variety” (p. 388) than the relations among the contents of episodic memories, which he believed to be organized chiefly along spatio-temporal dimensions. Since that time, a large number of theories and studies have focused on the contrast between semantic and associative relations because they are considered to be the two principle and distinguishable components of conceptual organization (Crutch & Warrington, 2010; Fischler, 1977; Hutchison, 2002; Shelton & Martin, 1992; Thompson-Schill, Kurtz, & Gabrielli, 1998; Yee, Overton, & Thompson-Schill, 2009). It has been a common working hypothesis in semantic memory research that these components are defined on orthogonal dimensions. Associative relatedness is defined typically in terms of stimulus-response combinations in a word association task (e.g., agony-pain; Nelson, McEvoy, & Schreiber, 1998). In fact, Nelson et al.’s word association norms, although not the Semantic & Associative Relations 3 sole source of word association norms in the literature, have been the most often used operationalization of association in memory research for at least the past decade. In contrast, semantic relatedness has typically been defined either as membership in the same superordinate category (e.g., horse-dog; Lupker, 1984), or as the degree to which the semantic features of two concepts overlap (horse-cow; Frenck-Mestre & Beuno, 1999). Often these two measures are treated as essentially the same, and indeed both are based on closeness in a representational structure, although featural overlap is more of a continuous dimension than is shared category. In this chapter, we outline our position concerning the relationship between association and meaning. Association in its general sense spatial and temporal co-occurrence in the world and language is an important driving force in learning, and this includes the formation of semantic representations. Furthermore, word association norms are an interesting and rich source of data. However, word associations on their own provide little if any insight into the relations that are encoded in semantic memory. Performance on word association norms is driven by meaningful semantic relations, and these relations are identifiable, and in many cases, quantifiable. We also argue that is not fruitful to attempt to understand semantic memory using a binary distinction between semantic similarity and word association (or even between semantic relatedness, broadly defined, vs. word association). On the one hand, the scope of semantic relations is much broader than similarity alone, and on the other hand, word associations are driven almost exclusively by semantic relations. Finally, a fruitful research strategy is to work toward understanding the relative importance or centrality of various types of semantic relations for various types of concepts. This approach, we believe, is the best path forward for understanding concepts and semantic memory. To provide evidence for these ideas, and to couch our arguments, we focus on four areas of research in which the semantics-word association dichotomy has played a major role. Section 4.1 deals with experiments regarding picture-word facilitation and interference. Section 4.2 concerns the DeeseRoediger-McDermott false memory paradigm. Section 4.3 focuses on semantic priming. Finally, Section 4.4 describes research concerning how the ability to learn word pairs develops across adolescence, and how this development crucially hinges on semantic knowledge, and the ability to employ that knowledge to make associations meaningful.

[1]  J. Deese On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  J. Deese The structure of associations in language and thought , 1966 .

[3]  M. Knapp,et al.  Association, synonymity, and directionality in false recognition. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[5]  Morris N. Eagle,et al.  Synonymity, antonymity, and association in false recognition responses , 1970 .

[6]  E. Tulving,et al.  Episodic and semantic memory , 1972 .

[7]  F. Berry,et al.  Stimulus selection in paired-associate learning: Consonant-triad versus word-triad paradigms , 1973 .

[8]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[9]  I. Fischler Semantic facilitation without association in a lexical decision task , 1977, Memory & cognition.

[10]  Richard R. Rosinski,et al.  Picture-word interference is semantically based. , 1977 .

[11]  W. F. Battig,et al.  Handbook of semantic word norms , 1978 .

[12]  R. Sperber,et al.  Semantic priming effects on picture and word processing , 1979 .

[13]  Kim Kirsner,et al.  Context and Repetition Effects in Lexical Decision and Recognition Memory. , 1982 .

[14]  W. D. Rohwer,et al.  Event knowledge, elaborative propensity, and the development of learning proficiency , 1982 .

[15]  W. Glaser,et al.  The time course of picture-word interference. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Pre- and postlexical loci of contextual effects on word recognition , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[17]  S. Lupker Semantic Priming without Association: A Second Look. , 1984 .

[18]  J. H. Neely,et al.  Dissociative episodic and semantic priming effects in episodic recognition and lexical decision tasks , 1985 .

[19]  David Hines,et al.  Automatic semantic priming: Effect of category exemplar level and word association level. , 1986 .

[20]  T. Beuhring,et al.  Developmental relationships among metamemory, elaborative strategy use, and associative memory☆ , 1987 .

[21]  T. Beuhring,et al.  Elaborative Propensities During Adolescence: The Relationships Among Memory Knowledge, Strategy Behavior, and Memory Performance , 1987 .

[22]  M. Bajo Semantic facilitation with pictures and words. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Lexical decision as an indirect test of memory: repetition priming and list-wide priming as a function of type of encoding , 1989 .

[24]  D Dagenbach,et al.  Adding new information to semantic memory: how much learning is enough to produce automatic priming? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  C. Burgess,et al.  Semantic and associative priming in the cerebral hemispheres: Some words do, some words don't … sometimes, some places , 1990, Brain and Language.

[26]  W. Heij,et al.  Categorical interference and associative priming in picture naming. , 1990 .

[27]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Spreading activation versus compound cue accounts of priming: mediated priming revisited. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  How semantic is automatic semantic priming , 1992 .

[29]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Repetition priming for newly formed and preexisting associations: perceptual and conceptual influences. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Accessing Different Types of Lexical Semantic Information: Evidence From Priming , 1995 .

[31]  Jason M. Blackwell,et al.  Memory Illusions: Recalling, Recognizing, and Recollecting Events that Never Occurred , 1996 .

[32]  Curt Burgess,et al.  Modelling Parsing Constraints with High-dimensional Context Space , 1997 .

[33]  T. Landauer,et al.  A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .

[34]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  J. Schrijnemakers,et al.  Adding new word associations to semantic memory: evidence for two interactive learning components. , 1997, Acta psychologica.

[36]  K. McRae,et al.  Automatic semantic similarity priming. , 1998 .

[37]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Effects of Semantic and Associative Relatedness on Automatic Priming , 1998 .

[38]  C. Frenck-Mestre,et al.  Semantic Features and Semantic Categories: Differences in Rapid Activation of the Lexicon , 1999, Brain and Language.

[39]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[40]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  Automatic Priming Effects for New Associations in Lexical Decision and Perceptual Identification , 1999, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[41]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Normsfor English Words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech Report C-1) , 1999 .

[42]  M. Lucas,et al.  Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[43]  S. Dennis,et al.  What is free association and what does it measure? , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[44]  J. Segui,et al.  Semantic and Associative Priming in Picture Naming , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[45]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  A brief history of memory research. , 2000 .

[46]  K. McRae,et al.  Integrating Verbs, Situation Schemas, and Thematic Role Concepts , 2001 .

[47]  K. McDermott,et al.  Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  K. Hutchison The effect of asymmetrical association on positive and negative semantic priming , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[49]  V. Reyna,et al.  Fuzzy-Trace Theory and False Memory , 2002 .

[50]  Keith A Hutchison,et al.  Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[51]  S. Bentin,et al.  Incidental formation of episodic associations: The importance of sentential context , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[52]  M. Garrett,et al.  Representing the meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[53]  Thomas A. Schreiber,et al.  The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[54]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[55]  Albert Costa,et al.  On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  W. Kintsch,et al.  High-Dimensional Semantic Space Accounts of Priming. , 2006 .

[57]  V. Reyna,et al.  Development of the false-memory illusion. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[58]  Alessandro Lenci,et al.  Semantic properties of word associations to Italian verbs , 2007 .

[59]  C. J. Brainerd,et al.  Semantic processing in “associative” false memory , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[60]  V. Reyna,et al.  Attention to global gist processing eliminates age effects in false memories. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[61]  David P Vinson,et al.  Semantic feature production norms for a large set of objects and events , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[62]  Shlomo Bentin,et al.  Word associations are formed incidentally during sentential semantic integration. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[63]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Perceptual simulation in conceptual combination: evidence from property generation. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[64]  Sharon L. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Looking for meaning: Eye movements are sensitive to overlapping semantic features, not association , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[65]  Christopher M. O'Connor,et al.  Conceptual Hierarchies in a Flat Attractor Network: Dynamics of Learning and Computations , 2009, Cogn. Sci..

[66]  Monica Baciu,et al.  The sensory-motor specificity of taxonomic and thematic conceptual relations: A behavioral and fMRI study , 2009, NeuroImage.

[67]  P. J. Brooks,et al.  Exploring the time course of semantic interference and associative priming in the picture–word interference task , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[68]  Mary Hare,et al.  Activating event knowledge , 2009, Cognition.

[69]  E. Warrington,et al.  The differential dependence of abstract and concrete words upon associative and similarity-based information: Complementary semantic interference and facilitation effects , 2010, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[70]  K. McRae,et al.  False Recall in the Deese–Roediger–McDermott Paradigm: The Roles of Gist and Associative Strength , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[71]  Michael N. Jones,et al.  Redundancy in Perceptual and Linguistic Experience: Comparing Feature-Based and Distributional Models of Semantic Representation , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[72]  Z. Estes,et al.  Thematic thinking : the apprehension and consequences of thematic relations , 2011 .

[73]  Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al.  Property generation reflects word association and situated simulation , 2011, Language and Cognition.