How Language Programs the Mind

Many animals can be trained to perform novel tasks. People, too, can be trained, but sometime in early childhood people transition from being trainable to something qualitatively more powerful-being programmable. We argue that such programmability constitutes a leap in the way that organisms learn, interact, and transmit knowledge, and that what facilitates or enables this programmability is the learning and use of language. We then examine how language programs the mind and argue that it does so through the manipulation of embodied, sensorimotor representations. The role language plays in controlling mental representations offers important insights for understanding its origin and evolution.

[1]  Susan Carey,et al.  Grey parrot number acquisition: The inference of cardinal value from ordinal position on the numeral list , 2012, Cognition.

[2]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Meaningless words promote meaningful categorization , 2014, Language and Cognition.

[3]  R. K. Thompson,et al.  Language-naive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) judge relations between relations in a conceptual matching-to-sample task. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[4]  S. Edelman,et al.  Canonical views in object representation and recognition , 1994, Vision Research.

[5]  Kim M. Curby,et al.  Conceptual knowledge attenuates viewpoint dependency in visual object recognition , 2013 .

[6]  L. Cohen,et al.  Labels can override perceptual categories in early infancy , 2008, Cognition.

[7]  M. Tomasello The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition , 2000 .

[8]  R. Millikan A common structure for concepts of individuals, stuffs, and real kinds: More Mama, more milk, and more mouse , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  L. Connell,et al.  Is a bear white in the woods? Parallel representation of implied object color during language comprehension , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  M. Tarr Rotating objects to recognize them: A case study on the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  E A Wasserman,et al.  Entropy and variability discrimination. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  D. Mirman,et al.  Linking language and categorization: Evidence from aphasia , 2013, Cortex.

[13]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Categorization is modulated by transcranial direct current stimulation over left prefrontal cortex , 2012, Cognition.

[14]  Antoni Gomila Comprar Verbal Minds. Language And The Architecture Of Cognition | Antoni Gomila | 9780123852007 | Elsevier Internacional , 2011 .

[15]  D. Kimbrough Oller,et al.  Vocabulary Learning in a Yorkshire Terrier: Slow Mapping of Spoken Words , 2012, PloS one.

[16]  Antoni Gomila Verbal minds : language and the architecture of cognition , 2012 .

[17]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition , 2008, Cognition.

[18]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions , 2012, Cortex.

[19]  Vladimir M. Sloutsky,et al.  The Role of Words in Cognitive Tasks: What, When, and How? , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[20]  S. Levinson,et al.  Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development , 2001 .

[21]  Irene M. Pepperberg,et al.  Abstract concepts: Data from a Grey parrot , 2013, Behavioural Processes.

[22]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Moving words: dynamic representations in language comprehension , 2004 .

[23]  Derek C. Penn,et al.  Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  V. Evans How Words Mean , 2009 .

[25]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Language can boost otherwise unseen objects into visual awareness , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Motor Resonance and Linguistic Focus , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[27]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Extracommunicative functions of language: Verbal interference causes selective categorization impairments , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  S. Levinson From outer to inner space: Linguistic categories and non-linguistic thinking , 1997 .

[29]  Jean M. Mandler,et al.  A summary of The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought , 2004 .

[30]  Marta Kutas,et al.  Empirically grounding grounded cognition: The case of color , 2014, NeuroImage.

[31]  W. N. Schoenfeld,et al.  Principles of Psychology , 2007 .

[32]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Topics in Cognitive Science , 2009 .

[33]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Head Up, Foot Down , 2008, Psychological science.

[34]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[35]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[36]  Edward A. Wasserman,et al.  Humans deploy diverse strategies in learning same–different discrimination tasks , 2013, Behavioural Processes.

[37]  Lila R. Gleitman,et al.  Why It Is Hard to Label Our Concepts. , 2004 .

[38]  F. Keil,et al.  Thinking Through Language , 2001 .

[39]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  G. Lupyan The conceptual grouping effect: Categories matter (and named categories matter more) , 2008, Cognition.

[41]  Paul R. Hays From the Jurassic dark: Linguistic relativity as evolutionary necessity , 2000 .

[42]  G. S. Marmor Age at onset of blindness and the development of the semantics of color names. , 1978, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  The role of language in multi-dimensional categorization: Evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation and exposure to verbal labels , 2014, Brain and Language.

[44]  A Baddeley,et al.  Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[45]  S. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Manual Experience Shapes Object Representations , 2013, Psychological science.

[46]  David Barner,et al.  Slow mapping: Color word learning as a gradual inductive process , 2013, Cognition.

[47]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Where are the concepts? What words can and can’t reveal , 2015 .

[48]  Phillip Wolff,et al.  Spatial language and the psychological reality of schematization , 2013, Cognitive Processing.

[49]  Angelo Cangelosi,et al.  Simulating the Evolution of Language , 2002, Springer London.

[50]  Gedeon O Deák,et al.  The development of cognitive flexibility and language abilities. , 2003, Advances in child development and behavior.

[51]  A. Whiten,et al.  Primate Calls, Human Language, and Nonverbal Communication [and Comments and Reply] , 1993, Current Anthropology.

[52]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain Signatures of Meaning Access in Action Word Recognition , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  Daniel Casasanto,et al.  ONLY Margolis — The Conceptual Mind El 19 All Concepts Are Ad Hoc Concepts , 2014 .

[54]  S. Thompson-Schill,et al.  The evocative power of words: activation of concepts by verbal and nonverbal means. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[55]  S. Engel Thought and Language , 1964 .

[56]  Nadja Althaus,et al.  Labels Direct Infants’ Attention to Commonalities during Novel Category Learning , 2014, PloS one.

[57]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[58]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Language Comprehenders Mentally Represent the Shapes of Objects , 2002, Psychological science.

[59]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning , 2002, Cognition.

[60]  A. Clark Magic Words: How Language Augments Human Computation , 1998 .

[61]  S. Edelman,et al.  Orientation dependence in the recognition of familiar and novel views of three-dimensional objects , 1992, Vision Research.

[62]  Marianella Casasola,et al.  Can language do the driving? The effect of linguistic input on infants' categorization of support spatial relations. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[63]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[64]  Caren M. Walker,et al.  Toddlers Infer Higher-Order Relational Principles in Causal Learning , 2014, Psychological science.

[65]  Alliston K. Reid,et al.  Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents , 2011, Behavioural Processes.

[66]  G. Lupyan From chair to "chair": a representational shift account of object labeling effects on memory. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[67]  S. Savage-Rumbaugh,et al.  Apes, Language, and the Human Mind , 1998 .

[68]  Melissa Bowerman,et al.  Roots of word learning , 2001 .

[69]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Redundant Spoken Labels Facilitate Perception of Multiple Items A417ra – Nt – Cla , 2022 .

[70]  Adele E. Goldberg Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[71]  Stevan Harnad,et al.  Cognition is categorization , 2005 .

[72]  David L. Faigman,et al.  Human category learning. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[73]  A. Cangelosi,et al.  Symbol grounding and the symbolic theft hypothesis , 2002 .

[74]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[75]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Action–effect learning in early childhood: does language matter? , 2010, Psychological research.

[76]  John Paul Minda,et al.  The Influence of Verbal and Nonverbal Processing on Category Learning , 2010 .

[77]  E. Pothos The rules versus similarity distinction. , 2005, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[78]  J. Call,et al.  Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for "Fast Mapping" , 2004, Science.

[79]  J. Elman On the Meaning of Words and Dinosaur Bones: Lexical Knowledge Without a Lexicon , 2009, Cogn. Sci..

[80]  Denis Mareschal,et al.  The perceptual origins of the abstract same/different concept in human infants , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[81]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[82]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Social learning of nut-cracking behavior in East African sanctuary-living chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.

[83]  B. Farell,et al.  Same-different judgments: a review of current controversies in perceptual comparisons. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[84]  M. Tarr,et al.  Mental rotation and orientation-dependence in shape recognition , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[85]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: when does language become a tool for categorization? , 2001, Cognition.

[86]  J. Fodor Lot 2: The Language of Thought Revisited , 2008 .

[87]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation , 2003 .

[88]  Ernst Cassirer,et al.  Review: An Essay on Man: An Introduction to a Philosophy of Human Culture , 1960 .

[89]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Understanding in an instant: Neurophysiological evidence for mechanistic language circuits in the brain , 2009, Brain and Language.

[90]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural foundations of imagery , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[91]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Schemata and Sequential Thought Processes in PDP Models , 1986 .

[92]  G. Lupyan,et al.  What makes words special? Words as unmotivated cues , 2015, Cognition.

[93]  V. Evans How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models, and Meaning Construction , 2009 .

[94]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[95]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Self-directed Speech Affects Visual Search Performance , 2022 .

[96]  Ursula Hess,et al.  Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science , 2017 .

[97]  Benjamin K. Bergen,et al.  Language comprehenders represent object distance both visually and auditorily , 2012, Language and Cognition.

[98]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Verbal and Nonverbal Cues Activate Concepts Differently, at Different Times , 2013, CogSci.

[99]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Language is not Just for Talking , 2007, Psychological science.

[100]  Larissa K. Samuelson,et al.  Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. , 2012, Psychological review.

[101]  G. Lupyan Chapter Seven – What Do Words Do? Toward a Theory of Language-Augmented Thought , 2012 .

[102]  Daniel C. Dennett,et al.  The Role of Language in Intelligence , 1994 .

[103]  B. Bergen Louder Than Words: The New Science of How the Mind Makes Meaning , 2012 .

[104]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Constraints on representational change: Evidence from children's drawing , 1990, Cognition.

[105]  J. Carroll,et al.  Language, Thought and Reality , 1957 .

[106]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Does language about similarity play a role in fostering similarity comparison in children? , 2009, Cognition.

[107]  Vladimir M. Sloutsky,et al.  From Perceptual Categories to Concepts: What Develops? , 2010, Cogn. Sci..