The role of inhibition in a spreading-activation model of language production. I. The psycholinguistic perspective

Spreading-activation models of language production are only workable to the extent that they manage to solve the “heat death” problem, i.e., the danger that too many nodes in the network are overactive at the same time. Therefore, a delicate balance between activational and deactivational forces has to be struck. Of the three prevailing dampening mechanisms of decay, self-inhibition and other-inhibition, the latter has been selected for closer scrutiny. The key proposal of this two-part article is that activation-based models of language production cannot afford to do without an inhibitory component, in particular lateral inhibition among nodes of the same level. Psycholinguistic evidence is reviewed in an attempt to insulate inhibitory from excitatory mechanisms. Although it is difficult in normal adult language use to distinguish between the effects of excessive activation and insufficient inhibition, some patterns from language acquisition and aphasia can be shown to follow from inhibitory rather than excitatory problems, thus demonstrating the reality of inhibition. In a system of activational and deactivational forces, other-inhibition is claimed to have the excitatory mechanism of syntax as its natural opponent. It is finally argued that other-inhibition offers an explanation for some puzzling findings from the experimental literature.

[1]  L. Hurwitz,et al.  On perseveration in aphasics. , 1967, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[3]  Lise Menn,et al.  PHONOTACTIC RULES IN BEGINING SPEECH , 1971 .

[4]  PHONEMATISCH-SYNTAKTISCHE APHASIE - Ein Sonderfall motorischer Aphasie bei einer zweisprachigen Patientin , 1971 .

[5]  E. Loftus Activation of Semantic Memory , 1973 .

[6]  George Branigan,et al.  Syllabic structure and the acquisition of consonants: The great conspiracy in word formation , 1976, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.

[7]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[8]  J. Duchan,et al.  Phonological analysis: a multifaceted approach. , 1978, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[9]  H. Whitaker,et al.  On Linguistic Perseveration , 1979 .

[10]  C. A. Becker Semantic context effects in visual word recognition: An analysis of semantic strategies , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[11]  A. Yamadori Verbal perseveration in aphasia , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[13]  Henry L. Roediger,et al.  Retrieval blocks in episodic and semantic memory. , 1982 .

[14]  Michel Paradis,et al.  Alternate antagonism with paradoxical translation behavior in two bilingual aphasic patients , 1982, Brain and Language.

[15]  B. Baars,et al.  Experimental verbal slip studies: A review and an editing model of language encoding , 1983 .

[16]  John R. Anderson A spreading activation theory of memory. , 1983 .

[17]  T A Blaxton,et al.  Inhibition from semantically related primes: Evidence of a category-specific inhibition , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[18]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  A Critique of Top-down Independent Levels Models of Speech Production: Evidence from Non-plan-Internal Speech Errors , 1984, Cogn. Sci..

[19]  Joseph Paul Stemberger,et al.  The lexicon in a model of language production , 1985 .

[20]  D. Meyer,et al.  Speech production: Motor programming of phonetic features , 1985 .

[21]  Interference between phonemes during phoneme monitoring: evidence for an interactive activation model of speech perception. , 1985 .

[22]  Jordan B. Pollack,et al.  Massively Parallel Parsing: A Strongly Interactive Model of Natural Language Interpretation , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[23]  James L. McClelland Putting Knowledge in its Place: A Scheme for Programming Parallel Processing Structures on the Fly , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[24]  T. Berg,et al.  Is voice a suprasegmental? , 1985 .

[25]  Donald G. MacKay,et al.  Self-Inhibition and the Disruptive Effects of Internal and External Feedback in Skilled Behavior* , 1986 .

[26]  K. Briand,et al.  Priming single digit numbers: automatic spreading activation dissipates as a function of semantic distance , 1986 .

[27]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[28]  L. Colombo Activation and inhibition with orthographically similar words. , 1986 .

[29]  Inhibiting word generation with word presentations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  K. Bock An Effect of the Accessibility of Word Forms on Sentence Structures , 1987 .

[31]  Thomas Berg,et al.  Die Abbildung des Sprachproduktionsprozesses in einem Aktivationsflußmodell: Untersuchungen an deutschen und englischen Versprechern , 1988 .

[32]  W. Levelt Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[33]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[34]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  Paragrammatisms: Syntactic disturbance or breakdown of control? , 1990, Cognition.

[35]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. , 1991 .

[36]  W. Levelt,et al.  Normal and deviant lexical processing: Reply to Dell and O'Seaghdha (1991). , 1991 .

[37]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .

[38]  G. Dell,et al.  Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: a comment on Levelt et al. (1991). , 1991, Psychological review.