This is your brain on Scrabble: Neural correlates of visual word recognition in competitive Scrabble players as measured during task and resting-state

Competitive Scrabble players devote considerable time to studying words and practicing Scrabble-related skills (e.g., anagramming). This training is associated with extraordinary performance in lexical decision, the standard visual word recognition task (Hargreaves, Pexman, Zdrazilova & Sargious, 2012). In the present study we investigated the neural consequences of this lexical expertise. Using both event-related and resting-state fMRI, we compared brain activity and connectivity in 12 competitive Scrabble experts with 12 matched non-expert controls. Results showed that when engaged in the lexical decision task (LDT), Scrabble experts made use of brain regions not generally associated with meaning retrieval in visual word recognition, but rather those associated with working memory and visual perception. The analysis of resting-state data also showed group differences, such that a different network of brain regions was associated with higher levels of Scrabble-related skill in experts than in controls.

[1]  B. Uttl,et al.  North American Adult Reading Test: Age Norms, Reliability, and Validity , 2002, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[2]  Anthony Randal McIntosh,et al.  Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: applications and advances , 2004, NeuroImage.

[3]  O. Spreen,et al.  A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .

[4]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[5]  Evelyne Marmèche,et al.  Effect of expertise acquisition on strategic perception: The example of chess , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  D. Plaut,et al.  Distributed circuits, not circumscribed centers, mediate visual recognition , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  David A. Medler,et al.  Distinct Brain Systems for Processing Concrete and Abstract Concepts , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Penny M. Pexman,et al.  How a hobby can shape cognition: visual word recognition in competitive Scrabble players , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[9]  C. Grady,et al.  Blood Oxygen Level-Dependent Signal Variability Is More than Just Noise , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Roland H. Grabner The role of intelligence for performance in the prototypical expertise domain of chess , 2014 .

[11]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  How chunks, long-term working memory and templates offer a cognitive explanation for neuroimaging data on expertise acquisition: A two-stage framework , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[12]  N. Kovacevic,et al.  Characterizing Functional Integrity: Intraindividual Brain Signal Variability Predicts Memory Performance in Patients with Medial Temporal Lobe Epilepsy , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  J HolcombPhillip,et al.  On the Time Course of Visual Word Recognition , 2006 .

[14]  E. M. Altmann,et al.  Deliberate practice: Is that all it takes to become an expert? , 2014 .

[15]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Yvette Hackett Does Practice Make Perfect? , 2003 .

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[19]  Gianpiero Monittola,et al.  Distinguishable neurofunctional effects of task practice and item practice in picture naming: A BOLD fMRI study in healthy subjects , 2013, Brain and Language.

[20]  Jodi D. Edwards,et al.  An fMRI investigation of strategies for word recognition. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[21]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  The architecture of the chess player׳s brain , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  A. R. McIntosh,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of event-related fMRI data using partial least squares , 2004, NeuroImage.

[23]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[24]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  S. Black,et al.  Network interactions explain effective encoding in the context of medial temporal damage in MCI , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[26]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Why expert performance is special and cannot be extrapolated from studies of performance in the general population: A response to criticisms. , 2014 .

[27]  B. Postle,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Is Critical for the Manipulation of Information in Working Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Xenophon Papademetris,et al.  More accurate Talairach coordinates for neuroimaging using non-linear registration , 2008, NeuroImage.

[29]  Cathy J. Price,et al.  A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading , 2012, NeuroImage.

[30]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words , 2012, Behavior research methods.

[31]  Mark J. Huff,et al.  An Abundance of Riches: Cross-Task Comparisons of Semantic Richness Effects in Visual Word Recognition , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[32]  Diane F Halpern,et al.  The world of competitive Scrabble: Novice and expert differences in visuopatial and verbal abilities. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[33]  Blair C. Armstrong,et al.  The what, when, where, and how of visual word recognition , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Peter A. Bandettini,et al.  Detection versus Estimation in Event-Related fMRI: Choosing the Optimal Stimulus Timing , 2002, NeuroImage.

[35]  Hsuan-Chih Chen,et al.  Brain Responses to Segmentally and Tonally Induced Semantic Violations in Cantonese , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  O. Spreen,et al.  Comprar A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests - Administration, Norms, and Commentary | Esther Strauss | 9780195159578 | Oxford University Press , 2009 .

[37]  John Jonides,et al.  How does practice makes perfect? , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  F. Bookstein,et al.  Neurobehavioral effects of prenatal alcohol: Part II. Partial least squares analysis. , 1989, Neurotoxicology and teratology.

[39]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Imageability ratings for 3,000 monosyllabic words , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[40]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[41]  Michael Erb,et al.  Mechanisms and neural basis of object and pattern recognition: a study with chess experts. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[42]  Yuan Li,et al.  Reduced caudate volume and enhanced striatal-DMN integration in chess experts , 2012, NeuroImage.

[43]  Malek Adjouadi,et al.  Comparative reliability analysis of publicly available software packages for automatic intracranial volume estimation , 2014, 2014 36th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[44]  F. Gobet,et al.  Functional cerebral reorganization: a signature of expertise? Reexamining Guida, Gobet, Tardieu, and Nicolas' (2012) two-stage framework , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[45]  J. V. Haxby,et al.  Spatial Pattern Analysis of Functional Brain Images Using Partial Least Squares , 1996, NeuroImage.

[46]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  On the Time Course of Visual Word Recognition: An Event-related Potential Investigation using Masked Repetition Priming , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[48]  S. Dehaene,et al.  How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language , 2010, Science.

[49]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  Benoit M. Dawant,et al.  Comparison of fMRI statistical software packages and strategies for analysis of images containing random and stimulus-correlated motion , 2007, Comput. Medical Imaging Graph..

[51]  S. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Reworking the language network , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  Aljoscha C. Neubauer,et al.  Superior performance and neural efficiency: The impact of intelligence and expertise , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[53]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[54]  Yasushi Hino,et al.  The impact of feedback semantics in visual word recognition: Number-of-features effects in lexical decision and naming tasks , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  D. Balota,et al.  Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  Daniel J. Acheson,et al.  New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[57]  Michael Tuffiash,et al.  Expert performance in SCRABBLE: implications for the study of the structure and acquisition of complex skills. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[58]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Learning to read improves the structure of the arcuate fasciculus. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Comparison of fMRI motion correction software tools , 2005, NeuroImage.

[60]  H. Simon,et al.  Perception in chess , 1973 .