Functional lateralization of temporoparietal junction – imitation inhibition, visual perspective‐taking and theory of mind
暂无分享,去创建一个
Geoffrey Bird | Caroline Catmur | Idalmis Santiesteban | G. Bird | C. Catmur | M. Banissy | Idalmis Santiesteban | Michael J. Banissy
[1] R. Saxe,et al. Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[2] M. Iacoboni,et al. The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[3] N. Rinehart,et al. Noninvasive stimulation of the temporoparietal junction: A systematic review , 2015, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[4] C. Caltagirone,et al. Asymmetry of Parietal Interhemispheric Connections in Humans , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] G. Bird,et al. Atypical Social Modulation of Imitation in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2011, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[6] N. Kanwisher,et al. New method for fMRI investigations of language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] M. Braga,et al. Exploratory Data Analysis , 2018, Encyclopedia of Social Network Analysis and Mining. 2nd Ed..
[8] J. Perner,et al. From infants’ to children's appreciation of belief , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[9] Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al. An Agent Independent Axis for Executed and Modeled Choice in Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2013, Neuron.
[10] C. Frith,et al. Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[11] I. Apperly. Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of "Theory of Mind" , 2010 .
[12] Jan K. Woike,et al. Introducing MASC: A Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition , 2006, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[13] Frank Van Overwalle,et al. Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.
[14] Jason P. Mitchell,et al. Mentalizing under Uncertainty: Dissociated Neural Responses to Ambiguous and Unambiguous Mental State Inferences , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[15] William A. Cunningham,et al. Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[16] Sukhvinder S. Obhi,et al. Task-dependent and distinct roles of the temporoparietal junction and inferior frontal cortex in the control of imitation , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[17] M. Brass,et al. The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[18] Geoffrey Bird,et al. Enhancing Social Ability by Stimulating Right Temporoparietal Junction Tdcs of Right Tpj , 2022 .
[19] R. Saxe,et al. Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[20] R. Dolan,et al. An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[21] Jason P. Mitchell. Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[22] Iroise Dumontheil,et al. Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. , 2010, Developmental science.
[23] C. Miniussi,et al. The Role of Timing in the Induction of Neuromodulation in Perceptual Learning by Transcranial Electric Stimulation , 2013, Brain Stimulation.
[24] A. Antal,et al. Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients , 2007, Brain Research Bulletin.
[25] Adam Qureshi,et al. Why are there limits on theory of mind use? Evidence from adults’ ability to follow instructions from an ignorant speaker , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[26] Marcel Brass,et al. Resisting motor mimicry: Control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions , 2010, Social neuroscience.
[27] M. Nitsche,et al. Safety criteria for transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in humans , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[28] M. Corbetta,et al. The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.
[29] M. Brass,et al. Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[30] D. Barr,et al. Taking Perspective in Conversation: The Role of Mutual Knowledge in Comprehension , 2000, Psychological science.
[31] W. Prinz,et al. Compatibility between Observed and Executed Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and Imitative Cues , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[32] C. Montag,et al. Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: Evidence from a video-based assessment , 2011, Psychiatry Research.
[33] Josef Perner,et al. Temporo-parietal Junction Activity in Theory-of-Mind Tasks: Falseness, Beliefs, or Attention , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[34] Martin Tegenthoff,et al. An rTMS study into self-face recognition using video-morphing technique. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[35] J. Perner,et al. Common brain areas engaged in false belief reasoning and visual perspective taking: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[36] Hauke R. Heekeren,et al. Neural correlates of social cognition in naturalistic settings: A model-free analysis approach , 2010, NeuroImage.
[37] J. Decety,et al. Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[38] Dana Samson,et al. Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[39] F. Van Overwalle. Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[40] Jessica A. Sommerville,et al. Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] Geoffrey Bird,et al. Training social cognition: From imitation to Theory of Mind , 2012, Cognition.
[42] Marcel Brass,et al. Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[43] R. Mar. The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.
[44] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] D. Samson,et al. There is more to mind reading than having theory of mind concepts: new directions in theory of mind research , 2010 .
[46] G. Bird,et al. Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in adolescents and adults , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[47] Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al. An Agent Independent Axis for Executed and Modeled Choice in Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.
[48] Jennifer L. Cook,et al. Task-relevance dependent gradients in medial prefrontal and temporoparietal cortices suggest solutions to paradoxes concerning self/other control , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[49] C. Heyes,et al. Mentalizing or submentalizing in a communication task? Evidence from autism and a camera control , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.