The process of lexical decision: More words about a parallel distributed processing model.

Central to Seidenberg and McClelland's (1989) parallel distributed processing (PDP) model's account of lexical decision is the assumption that performance is determined by the amount of overlap in the distribution of the orthographic error scores between words and nonwords. The present experiment demonstrates lexical decision performance that is independent of the distribution of orthographic error scores between words and nonwords. Furthermore, the orthographic error scores from the model capture no variance in the reaction time data to words, even in a condition in which the orthographic overlap between words and nonwords is minimal

[1]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[2]  John J. L. Morton,et al.  Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .

[3]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  H. Rubenstein,et al.  Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition , 1971 .

[5]  D. Meyer,et al.  Attention and Performance XIV , 1973 .

[6]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[7]  P. H. Lindsay Human Information Processing , 1977 .

[8]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[9]  A. J. Marcel,et al.  Aphasia, Dyslexia and the Phonological Coding of Written Words , 1977 .

[10]  P. E. Morris,et al.  Practical aspects of memory , 1980 .

[11]  J. Requin Attention and Performance VII , 1980 .

[12]  Derek Besner,et al.  Reading for Meaning: The Effects of Concurrent Articulation* , 1981 .

[13]  Debra V. Mcquade,et al.  Variable Reliance on Phonological Information in Visual Word Recognition , 1981 .

[14]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[15]  L Pring,et al.  Phonological codes and functional spelling units: Reality and implications , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  R W Schvaneveldt,et al.  An activation--verification model for letter and word recognition: the word-superiority effect. , 1982, Psychological review.

[17]  Derek Besner,et al.  Basic processes in reading: Two phonological codes. , 1982 .

[18]  J. Mitterer There are at least two kinds of poor readers: whole-word poor readers and recoding poor readers. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.

[19]  E. Pedhazur Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research: Explanation and Prediction , 1982 .

[20]  D. Besner,et al.  Suedohomofoan effects in visual word recognition: evidence for phonological processing. , 1983, Canadian journal of psychology.

[21]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Spelling-sound effects in reading: Time-course and decision criteria , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[22]  G. E. MacKinnon,et al.  Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice , 1985 .

[23]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[24]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Language perception and production , 1987 .

[25]  Derek Besner,et al.  Phonology, Lexical Access in Reading, and Articulatory Suppression: A Critical Review , 1987 .

[26]  D. Besner,et al.  Reading pseudohomophones: Implications for models of pronunciation assembly and the locus of word-frequency effects in naming. , 1987 .

[27]  G. C. Orden A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading , 1987 .

[28]  David Howard,et al.  Three ways for understanding written words, and their use in two contrasting cases of surface dyslexia (together with an odd routine for making 'orthographic' errors in oral word production). , 1987 .

[29]  Derek Besner,et al.  Word recognition and identification: Do word-frequency effects reflect lexical access? , 1988 .

[30]  G. Underwood,et al.  Strategical invariance in lexical access: The reappearance of the pseudohomophone effect. , 1988 .

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[32]  M. L. Millis,et al.  The effect of polysemy on lexical decision time: Now you see it, now you don’t , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[33]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Visual word recognition and pronunciation: a computational model and its implications , 1989 .

[34]  James L. McClelland,et al.  More Words but Still No Lexicon: Reply to Besner et al. (1990) , 1990 .

[35]  Ken N. Seergobin,et al.  On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? , 1990 .

[36]  Leslie Henderson,et al.  Morphologically based frequency effects in the recognition of irregularly inflected verbs , 1990 .

[37]  Michael Garman,et al.  Psycholinguistics: Accessing the mental lexicon , 1990 .

[38]  D. Besner,et al.  Visual Word Recognition: Evidence for Strategic Control of Lexical and Nonlexical Routines in Oral Reading , 1991 .

[39]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Lexical Representation and Process , 1991 .

[40]  Tim Shallice,et al.  EFFECTS OF WORD ABSTRACTNESS IN A CONNECTIONIST MODEL OF DEEP DYSLEXIA , 1991 .

[41]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia , 1991 .