CONNECTIONS AND DISCONNECTIONS: A CONNECTIONIST ACCOUNT OF SURFACE DYSLEXIA
暂无分享,去创建一个
James L. McClelland | David C. Plaut | Marlene Behrmann | Mark S. Seidenberg | Karalyn Patterson | John R. Hodges | D. Plaut | M. Seidenberg | M. Behrmann | J. Hodges | K. Patterson | Departmentof Psychology | Departmentof Psychology
[1] James L. McClelland,et al. Connections and disconnections: Acquired dyslexia in a computational model of reading processes. , 1989 .
[2] J. Marshall,et al. Patterns of paralexia: a psycholinguistic approach. , 1995, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[3] J. Hodges,et al. Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[4] E. Warrington,et al. Semantic memory and reading abilities: A case report , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[5] E. Funnell,et al. Response Biases in Oral Reading: An Account of the Co occurrence of Surface Dyslexia and Semantic Dementia , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[6] E. Warrington,et al. Phonological Reading: Phenomena and Paradoxes , 1986, Cortex.
[7] J. Hodges,et al. The relationship between comprehension and oral reading in progressive fluent aphasia , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[8] Janice Kay,et al. One Process, Not Two, in Reading Aloud: Lexical Analogies Do the Work of Non-Lexical Rules , 1981 .
[9] Paul W. B. Atkins,et al. Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .
[10] G. C. Orden,et al. Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[12] Tim Shallice,et al. Single and multiple component central dyslexic syndromes , 1995 .
[13] S. Sasanuma,et al. Progressive aphasia and surface Alexia in Japanese , 1995 .
[14] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Semantic effects in single-word naming. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[15] M. Coltheart,et al. Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. , 1994 .
[16] E. Funnell. Phonological processes in reading: new evidence from acquired dyslexia. , 1983, British journal of psychology.
[17] James L. McClelland,et al. Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. , 1987 .
[18] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.
[19] J. Hodges,et al. Deterioration of word meaning: Implications for reading , 1992, Neuropsychologia.
[20] D. Besner,et al. Visual Word Recognition: Evidence for Strategic Control of Lexical and Nonlexical Routines in Oral Reading , 1991 .
[21] John A. Bullinaria,et al. Connectionist Models of Reading: Incorporating Semantics , 1996 .
[22] M Coltheart,et al. Bilingual Biscriptal Deep Dyslexia , 1984, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[23] Alan H. Kawamoto,et al. Pronunciation of homographs , 1992 .
[24] S. Black,et al. Semantic encoding of pictures and words: Some neuropsychological observations , 1988 .
[25] K. Paap,et al. Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race , 1991 .
[26] M Behrmann,et al. Frequency and consistency effects in a pure surface dyslexic patient. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] A. Ellis,et al. Semantic loss without surface dyslexia , 1995 .
[28] H. Kucera,et al. Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .
[29] T. Shallice,et al. Reading without Semantics , 1983 .
[30] H. Coslett,et al. Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia , 1994 .
[31] Ken N. Seergobin,et al. On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? , 1990 .
[32] S. Breedin. Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia , 1994 .
[33] J. Hodges,et al. Charting the progression in semantic dementia: implications for the organisation of semantic memory. , 1995, Memory.
[34] M. Behrmann,et al. Surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: dual routes, single lexicon , 1992 .
[35] L. Henderson. Orthography and Word Recognition in Reading , 1982 .
[36] R. Glushko. The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .
[37] G. Humphreys,et al. Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[38] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output: Evidence from a category-specific semantic deficit , 1991, Brain and Language.
[39] E. Funnell. Phonological processes in reading: new evidence from acquired dyslexia. , 1983 .
[40] M. Coltheart,et al. Surface dyslexia. , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[41] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition , 1984 .
[42] D. Howard,et al. Abstract word anomia , 1995 .
[44] Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al. Parallel Networks that Learn to Pronounce English Text , 1987, Complex Syst..
[45] J. Hodges,et al. Progressive pure anomia: Insufficient activation of phonology by meaning , 1995 .
[46] J R Hodges,et al. Nonfluent progressive aphasia and semantic dementia: A comparative neuropsychological study , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[47] M. Coltheart,et al. Varieties of developmental dyslexia , 1993, Cognition.