The effects of saliency and task difficulty on visual search performance in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease

We asked whether the poor performance on visual search tasks typical of patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the result of a selective deficit in the ability to shift attention from item to item, or the consequence of an inefficient processing of each item within the search set. We attempted to manipulate the ease of attention shifting and item processing in a visual search task by manipulating target salience and task difficulty, respectively. Significant effects of both target saliency and task difficulty for both AD patients and age-matched controls were obtained, with the AD group displaying greater effects of both of these manipulations than the controls. This interaction remained even when the reaction time data were log-transformed to account for the overall slower reaction times of the AD group. We conclude that inefficiency in visual search tasks in AD probably represents the product of both attention shifting and target processing factors.

[1]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[2]  R Parasuraman,et al.  Controlling the focus of spatial attention during visual search: effects of advanced aging and Alzheimer disease. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[3]  Robert J Snowden,et al.  Spatial shifts in visual attention in normal ageing and dementia of the Alzheimer type , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  M. Bravo,et al.  The role of attention in different visual-search tasks , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  W. Jagust,et al.  Clinical Studies of Cerebral Blood Flow in Alzheimer's Disease , 1997, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Zhaoping Li A saliency map in primary visual cortex , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  R. Parasuraman,et al.  The apolipoprotein E gene, attention, and brain function. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[9]  H. Braak,et al.  Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes , 2004, Acta Neuropathologica.

[10]  J. Sharpe,et al.  Saccadic eye movement dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[11]  D. Price,et al.  Synapse loss in the temporal lobe in Alzheimer's disease , 1993, Annals of neurology.

[12]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Alterations of visual search strategy in Alzheimer's disease and aging. , 2000, Neuropsychology.

[13]  J. Haxby,et al.  Attention and brain function in Alzheimer's disease: A review. , 1993 .

[14]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[15]  G. Alexander,et al.  Selective impairment of spatial attention during visual search in Alzheimer's disease , 1995, Neuroreport.

[16]  J. Palmer Set-size effects in visual search: The effect of attention is independent of the stimulus for simple tasks , 1994, Vision Research.

[17]  H. Coslett,et al.  Narrowing the spotlight: A visual attentional disorder in presumed alzheimer's disease , 1995 .

[18]  A Tales,et al.  Visual search in Alzheimer’s disease: a deficiency in processing conjunctions of features , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  J. Hodges,et al.  Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer's disease. A critical review. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[21]  Mark Nawrot,et al.  Vision and cognition in Alzheimer’s disease , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  G. Leuba,et al.  Visual cortex in Alzheimer's disease: Occurencee of neuronal death and glial proliferation, and correlation with pathological hallmarks , 1994, Neurobiology of Aging.

[23]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.

[24]  M. Carrasco,et al.  The eccentricity effect: Target eccentricity affects performance on conjunction searches , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  Bradley T. Hyman,et al.  Neurofibrillary tangles but not senile plaques parallel duration and severity of Alzheimer's disease , 1992, Neurology.

[26]  S. Sternberg Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. , 1969, American scientist.

[27]  M Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  A. Treisman Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[29]  R. Nebes,et al.  Generalized cognitive slowing and severity of dementia in Alzheimer's disease: implications for the interpretation of response-time data. , 1992, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[30]  R. Parasuraman,et al.  Acetylcholine affects the spatial scale of attention: evidence from Alzheimer's disease. , 2000, Neuropsychology.

[31]  U. Neisser VISUAL SEARCH. , 1964, Scientific American.

[32]  J. Foster Selective attention in Alzheimer's disease. , 2001, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[33]  Hans Förstl,et al.  Clinical features of Alzheimer’s disease , 1999, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.

[35]  J. Muir,et al.  Visuospatial Attention: The Role of Target Contrast and Task Difficulty when Assessing the Effects of Cues , 2001, Perception.

[36]  T. Salthouse A Theory of Cognitive Aging , 1985 .

[37]  D. Stuss,et al.  Aging and visual search: Generalized cognitive slowing or selective deficit in attention? , 1995 .

[38]  D. Stuss,et al.  Visual attention deficits in Alzheimer's disease: simple versus conjoined feature search. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[39]  R. Kuljiš,et al.  Lesions in the Pulvinar in Patients with Alzheimer's Disease , 1994, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[40]  G. Alexander,et al.  Longitudinal PET Evaluation of Cerebral Metabolic Decline in Dementia: A Potential Outcome Measure in Alzheimer's Disease Treatment Studies. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[41]  Richard A. Armstrong,et al.  Neuropathological changes in the visual cortex in Alzheimer's disease , 1990 .

[42]  Piero Antuono,et al.  Cytochrome oxidase in Alzheimer's disease: Biochemical, histochemical, and immunohistochemical analyses of the visual and other systems , 1997, Vision Research.

[43]  Alterations of visual search strategy in Alzheimer's disease and aging. , 2000 .

[44]  D Kömpf,et al.  Eye movement dysfunction in dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1995, Dementia.

[45]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Alzheimer disease constricts the dynamic range of spatial attention in visual search , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Richard Hollister,et al.  Neuronal loss correlates with but exceeds neurofibrillary tangles in Alzheimer's disease , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[47]  J. Meere The role of attention. , 2002 .

[48]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Decision-making and attentional processes in ageing and in dementia of the Alzheimer's type , 1994 .

[49]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Visual Search for Feature and Conjunction Targets with an Attention Deficit , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.