Expert voices in echo chambers: effects of source expertise indicators on exposure to diverse opinions

We studied how a source expertise indicator impacted users' information seeking behavior when using a system aggregating diverse opinions, and how it interacted with a source position indicator to shape users' selectivity of information. We found that, for both attitude consistent and inconsistent information, the expertise indicator increased the selection of sources indicated to have high expertise and decreased that of low expertise. Moreover, when both source expertise and position indicators were present, users' selective exposure tendency, i.e., preferential selection of attitude consistent sources over inconsistent ones, decreased among expert sources. Moreover, we found that the expertise indicator could benefit encouraging common ground seeking with different others by increasing the agreement with, and perceived expertise of inconsistent sources indicated to be experts. Design implications for moderating selective exposure by highlighting the utility of dissonant information were discussed.

[1]  S. Bochner,et al.  Communicator discrepancy, source credibility, and opinion change. , 1966 .

[2]  Miriam J. Metzger Making sense of credibility on the Web: Models for evaluating online information and recommendations for future research , 2007, J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol..

[3]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[4]  Kate Ehrlich,et al.  Making sense of strangers' expertise from signals in digital artifacts , 2009, CHI.

[5]  Brandon Van Der Heide,et al.  Self-Generated Versus Other-Generated Statements and Impressions in Computer-Mediated Communication , 2009, Commun. Res..

[6]  Seungwoo Kang,et al.  NewsCube: delivering multiple aspects of news to mitigate media bias , 2009, CHI.

[7]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  Communicator credibility and communication discrepancy as determinants of opinion change. , 1963 .

[8]  L. Cronbach Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests , 1951 .

[9]  Dieter Frey,et al.  Recent Research on Selective Exposure to Information , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[10]  J. Walther Computer-Mediated Communication , 1996 .

[11]  Dan Roth,et al.  BiasTrust: teaching biased users about controversial topics , 2012, CIKM '12.

[12]  S. Chaiken Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. , 1980 .

[13]  Alan Borning,et al.  Supporting reflective public thought with considerit , 2012, CSCW.

[14]  R. Kelly Garrett,et al.  Resisting Political Fragmentation on the Internet , 2011, Daedalus.

[15]  C. D. Ward,et al.  Better Liked than Right , 1980 .

[16]  Wai-Tat Fu,et al.  Can you hear me now?: mitigating the echo chamber effect by source position indicators , 2014, CSCW.

[17]  M. Birnbaum,et al.  Source Credibility in Social Judgment : Bias , Expertise , and the Judge ' s Point of View , 1979 .

[18]  Kimberly A. Cameron,et al.  Strategies for Resisting Persuasion , 2003 .

[19]  S. Shyam Sundar,et al.  Using interface cues in online health community boards to change impressions and encourage user contribution , 2011, CHI.

[20]  John Riedl,et al.  Is seeing believing?: how recommender system interfaces affect users' opinions , 2003, CHI '03.

[21]  J. Freedman,et al.  SELECTIVE EXPOSURE TO INFORMATION: A CRITICAL REVIEW , 1967 .

[22]  N. Krämer,et al.  Information Selection in the Blogosphere: The Effect of Expertise, Community Rating, and Age , 2010 .

[23]  Sean A. Munson,et al.  Presenting diverse political opinions: how and how much , 2010, CHI.

[24]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Source factors and the elaboration likelihood model of persuasion , 1984 .

[25]  William Allen,et al.  The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness , 1953 .

[26]  Jagadeesh Gokhale,et al.  Should Social Security Be Privatized , 1995 .

[27]  B. Sternthal,et al.  Highly Credible Sources: Persuasive Facilitators or Persuasive Liabilities? , 1977 .