A Common Capacity Limitation for Response and Item Selection in Working Memory
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[2] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory , and Cognition Exceptions to the PRP Effect ? A Comparison of Prepared and Unconditioned Reflexes , 2014 .
[3] Richard Schweickert,et al. A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a stroop task , 1978 .
[4] Wilfried Kunde,et al. The locus of tool-transformation costs. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] K. Oberauer. Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[6] V. Franz,et al. Grasping for parsimony: Do some motor actions escape dorsal processing? , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[7] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Design for a working memory. , 2009 .
[8] W. Kunde,et al. On the costs of refocusing items in working memory: A matter of inhibition or decay? , 2008, Memory.
[9] N. Cowan. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[10] R. Dell’Acqua,et al. The Demonstration of Short-Term Consolidation , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[11] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Analogous mechanisms of selection and updating in declarative and procedural working memory: Experiments and a computational model , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.
[12] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Processing of representations in declarative and procedural working memory , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[13] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Selection of objects and tasks in working memory , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[14] Markus Janczyk,et al. The benefit of no choice: goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing , 2015, Psychological research.
[15] Ann Reynolds,et al. The locus of redundant-targets and nontargets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Markus Janczyk,et al. Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[17] H Garavan,et al. Co-ordination within and between verbal and visuospatial working memory: network modulation and anterior frontal recruitment , 2003, NeuroImage.
[18] C. W. Telford. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .
[19] K. Oberauer. The focus of attention in working memory—from metaphors to mechanisms , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[20] Hugh Garavan,et al. Serial attention within working memory , 1998, Memory & cognition.
[21] Alessandra S. Souza,et al. Interference within and between declarative and procedural representations in working memory , 2014 .
[22] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[23] Action selection by temporally distal goal states , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[24] Markus Janczyk,et al. Confidence intervals for two sample means: Calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules , 2013, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[25] J. Grabowski,et al. The focus of attention in working memory: Evidence from a word updating task , 2011, Memory.