Experimental manipulation of attentional biases in heavy drinkers: do the effects generalise?
暂无分享,去创建一个
Matt Field | M. Field | T. Duka | B. Eastwood | Theodora Duka | Mary Santarcangelo | Brian Eastwood | Robert Child | Melanie Gayton | Mary Santarcangelo | R. Child | Melanie Gayton
[1] B. Jones,et al. Social users of alcohol and cannabis who detect substance-related changes in a change blindness paradigm report higher levels of use than those detecting substance-neutral changes , 2002, Psychopharmacology.
[2] Reinout W Wiers,et al. Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. , 2007, Addiction.
[3] Frank Ryan,et al. Detected, selected, and sometimes neglected: cognitive processing of cues in addiction. , 2002, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[4] K. Berridge,et al. The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction , 1993, Brain Research Reviews.
[5] T. Duka,et al. The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[6] M. Posner,et al. Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.
[7] G. Yeates,et al. Effects of alcohol cues on cognitive processing in heavy and light drinkers. , 1999, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[8] J. O'Connor. Sensible drinking. , 1994, World health forum.
[9] Paul Christiansen,et al. Delay discounting and the alcohol Stroop in heavy drinking adolescents. , 2007, Addiction.
[10] A. Dickinson,et al. Explicit knowledge of stimulus–outcome contingencies and stimulus control of selective attention and instrumental action in human smoking behaviour , 2005, Psychopharmacology.
[11] T. Duka,et al. Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers , 2001, Psychopharmacology.
[12] K. Mogg,et al. Attention to drug-related cues in drug abuse and addiction: component processes , 2006 .
[13] K. Mogg,et al. Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: the roles of initial orienting and maintained attention , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[14] R. Bevins,et al. Conditioned place preference: what does it add to our preclinical understanding of drug reward? , 2000, Psychopharmacology.
[15] M. Munafo,et al. Cognition and addiction , 2006 .
[16] A. Tomie. Locating Reward Cue at Response Manipulandum (CAM) Induces Symptoms of Drug Abuse , 1996, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[17] P. Willner,et al. A comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires. , 1998, Addiction.
[18] Maristela Monteiro,et al. AUDIT - The alcohol use disorders identification test: guidelines for use in primary care. , 2001 .
[19] K. Mogg,et al. Attentional and approach biases for smoking cues in smokers: an investigation of competing theoretical views of addiction , 2005, Psychopharmacology.
[20] M. Field,et al. Cues paired with a low dose of alcohol acquire conditioned incentive properties in social drinkers , 2001, Psychopharmacology.
[21] Matt Field,et al. Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. , 2003, Addiction.
[22] K. Mogg,et al. Craving and cognitive biases for alcohol cues in social drinkers. , 2005, Alcohol and alcoholism.
[23] Xavier Noël,et al. Time course of attention for alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholic patients: the role of initial orienting. , 2006, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.
[24] C. MacLeod,et al. Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. , 2002, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[25] L. Hogarth,et al. Human nicotine conditioning requires explicit contingency knowledge: is addictive behaviour cognitively mediated? , 2006, Psychopharmacology.
[26] S. Tiffany,et al. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. , 1990, Psychological review.
[27] I. Albery,et al. Selective attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli in problem drinkers and non-problem drinkers. , 2001, Addiction.
[28] S. Shiffman,et al. Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. , 2003, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.
[29] Ingmar H. A. Franken,et al. Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches , 2003, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
[30] M. R. Kristian,et al. Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers' treatment outcome. , 2002, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[31] Javad Salehi Fadardi,et al. The addiction-stroop test: Theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.
[32] R. Wiers,et al. How to Change Implicit Drug Use-Related Cognitions in Prevention: A Transdisciplinary Integration of Findings from Experimental Psychopathology, Social Cognition, Memory, and Experimental Learning Psychology , 2004, Substance use & misuse.
[33] K. Mogg,et al. Selective processing of cannabis cues in regular cannabis users. , 2006, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[34] H. Nordby,et al. Alcoholics' selective attention to alcohol stimuli: automated processing? , 2000, Journal of studies on alcohol.
[35] Thomas Babor,et al. Brief intervention for hazardous and harmful drinking: a manual for use in primary care. , 2001 .
[36] T. Palfai,et al. The search for new ways to change implicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy drinkers. , 2006, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.
[37] R. Wiers,et al. Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction , 2006 .
[38] D. Kavanagh,et al. Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: the elaborated intrusion theory of desire. , 2005, Psychological review.
[39] Karin Mogg,et al. Selective processing of smoking-related cues in smokers: manipulation of deprivation level and comparison of three measures of processing bias , 2002, Journal of psychopharmacology.
[40] W. Thalheimer,et al. How to calculate effect sizes from published research: A simplified methodology , 2002 .
[41] M. Field,et al. Smoking expectancy mediates the conditioned responses to arbitrary smoking cues , 2001, Behavioural pharmacology.
[42] M. Field,et al. Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol , 2005, Psychopharmacology.
[43] S. Tiffany,et al. Applying extinction research and theory to cue-exposure addiction treatments. , 2002, Addiction.
[44] W. van den Brink,et al. Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following treatment. , 2006, Addiction.
[45] R. Anton,et al. The Obsessive Compulsive Drinking Scale: a self-rated instrument for the quantification of thoughts about alcohol and drinking behavior. , 1995, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.