Covert orienting to central visual cues and sport practice relations in the development of visual attention.

This study examines the development of controlled covert orienting of visual attention, according to the age and the level of performance of tennis players. Practicers and nonpracticers age 13, 16, and 25 participated in a covert orienting task. On each trial, subjects responded to a target which appeared in one of four locations arrayed horizontally across the display. Three central cues were used: neutral, the cue did not specify in which location the target would appear; valid, the target was present in the cued location on 80% of the trials; invalid, the target was present in one of the uncued locations on the remaining 20% of the trials. The results showed that (1) practicers were not faster than nonpracticers in processing the signals, (2) observers of all ages oriented attention voluntarily to the cued locations, (3) central cues had a smaller effect on older and practiced subjects, and (4) developmental and sport practice factors had similar effects on orienting efficiency. The implications of these findings for theories of attentional development are discussed.

[1]  J. Pascual-Leone Compounds, confounds, and models in developmental information processing: A reply to Trabasso and Foellinger ☆ , 1978 .

[2]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[3]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Movements of attention in the three spatial dimensions and the meaning of “neutral” cues , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  D. Lane,et al.  The Development of Selective Attention. , 1982 .

[7]  P. Siple,et al.  Spatial constraints on the voluntary control of attention across visual space. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[8]  J. Enns,et al.  A developmental study of covert orienting to peripheral visual cues. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  D. LaBerge,et al.  Variations in size of the visual field in which targets are presented: An attentional range effect , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Y. Tsal Movements of attention across the visual field , 1983 .

[11]  M. Chi Short-term memory limitations in children: Capacity or processing deficits? , 1976, Memory & cognition.

[12]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[13]  J. Stein,et al.  Information processing in sport and 'orienting of attention'. , 1991 .

[14]  C. Possamaï,et al.  Relationship between inhibition and facilitation following a visual cue. , 1986 .

[15]  J T Enns,et al.  Selective attention in young children: the relations between visual search, filtering, and priming. , 1987, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[16]  G. Shulman,et al.  Moving attention through visual space. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  H. Egeth,et al.  Failure of spatial selectivity in vision , 1978 .

[18]  B. Nettleton Flexibility of Attention and Elite Athletes' Performance in ‘Fast-Ball-Games’ , 1986 .

[19]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Allocation of attention in the visual field. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  W. Surwillo Developmental Changes in the Speed of Information Processing , 1977 .

[21]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Acquisition of motor skills: information processing differences between children and adults. , 1980, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  A. Bonnel,et al.  Early Modulation of Visual Input: A Study of Attentional Strategies , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[24]  Mitsuo Ikeda,et al.  Influence of foveal load on the functional visual field , 1975 .

[25]  R. Remington,et al.  Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[27]  J. Enns,et al.  Relations between covert orienting and filtering in the development of visual attention. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[28]  Vincent Nougier,et al.  Orienting of attention with highly skilled athletes. , 1989 .

[29]  G. Humphreys On Varying the Span of Visual Attention: Evidence for Two Modes of Spatial Attention , 1981 .

[30]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Temporal limits of human information processing: A developmental study. , 1974 .

[31]  J. E. Clark Developmental differences in response processing. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  M. Haith,et al.  Peripheral vision: the effects of developmental, perceptual, and cognitive factors. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[33]  S W Keele,et al.  Explorations of individual differences relevant to high level skill. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  S M Anstis,et al.  Letter: A chart demonstrating variations in acuity with retinal position. , 1974, Vision research.

[35]  J. Jonides,et al.  On the Cost and Benefit of Cost and Benefit , 1984 .

[36]  J. Enns,et al.  Developmental changes in selective and integrative visual attention. , 1985, Journal of experimental child psychology.