Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources.

The numerosity of small numbers of objects, up to about four, can be rapidly appraised without error, a phenomenon known as subitizing. Larger numbers can either be counted, accurately but slowly, or estimated, rapidly but with errors. There has been some debate as to whether subitizing uses the same or different mechanisms than those of higher numerical ranges and whether it requires attentional resources. We measure subjects' accuracy and precision in making rapid judgments of numerosity for target numbers spanning the subitizing and estimation ranges while manipulating the attentional load, both with a spatial dual task and the "attentional blink" dual-task paradigm. The results of both attentional manipulations were similar. In the high-load attentional condition, Weber fractions were similar in the subitizing (2-4) and estimation (5-7) ranges (10-15%). In the low-load and single-task condition, Weber fractions substantially improved in the subitizing range, becoming nearly error-free, while the estimation range was relatively unaffected. The results show that the mechanisms operating over the subitizing and estimation ranges are not identical. We suggest that pre-attentive estimation mechanisms works at all ranges, but in the subitizing range, attentive mechanisms also come into play.

[1]  D. Burr,et al.  Vision and Audition Do Not Share Attentional Resources in Sustained Tasks , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[2]  Bahador Bahrami,et al.  A Candidate for the Attentional Bottleneck: Set-size Specific Modulation of the Right TPJ during Attentive Enumeration , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[4]  K. Verfaillie,et al.  Face inversion impairs holistic perception: evidence from gaze-contingent stimulation. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[5]  D. Melcher A shared sensorimotor map for visual memory, counting and trans-saccadic perception , 2010 .

[6]  G. Alvarez,et al.  Number estimation relies on a set of segmented objects , 2009, Cognition.

[7]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Jun Zhang,et al.  Connectedness affects dot numerosity judgment: Implications for configural processing , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  D. Burr,et al.  Vision senses number directly. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[10]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[11]  Bahador Bahrami,et al.  Modulating Attentional Load Affects Numerosity Estimation: Evidence against a Pre-Attentive Subitizing Mechanism , 2008, PloS one.

[12]  Chang Liu,et al.  Can subitizing survive the attentional blink? An ERP study , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[13]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Does Subitizing Reflect Numerical Estimation? , 2022 .

[14]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures , 2008, Science.

[15]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Evidence for counting in insects , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[16]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  The role of attention in subitizing: Is the magical number 1? , 2008 .

[17]  Christian N. L. Olivers,et al.  Subitizing requires attention , 2008 .

[18]  Henry Railo,et al.  The role of attention in subitizing , 2008, Cognition.

[19]  D. Burr,et al.  A Visual Sense of Number , 2007, Current Biology.

[20]  Fei Xu,et al.  Linking Visual Attention and Number Processing in the Brain: The Role of the Temporo-parietal Junction in Small and Large Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Number Comparison , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  I. Pepperberg Grey parrot numerical competence: a review , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[22]  David Alais,et al.  Separate attentional resources for vision and audition , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[24]  E. Miller,et al.  Analog Numerical Representations in Rhesus Monkeys: Evidence for Parallel Processing , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton–top tamarins , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[26]  J. Ross Visual Discrimination of Number without Counting , 2003, Perception.

[27]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Representation of the Quantity of Visual Items in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 2002, Science.

[28]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Tanji,et al.  Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey , 2002, Nature.

[30]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Are Subitizing and Counting Implemented as Separate or Functionally Overlapping Processes? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[31]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[32]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[33]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[36]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  F. Campbell,et al.  The Magic Number 4 ± 0: A New Look at Visual Numerosity Judgements , 1976, Perception.

[38]  E. L. Kaufman,et al.  The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.

[39]  Fei Xu,et al.  Number sense in human infants. , 2005, Developmental science.

[40]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Neural Evidence Linking Visual Object Enumeration and Attention , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[42]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[43]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition? , 1992, Psychological research.

[44]  K L Shapiro,et al.  Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[46]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[47]  J. B.,et al.  The Power of Numerical Discrimination , 1871, Nature.